Cargando…

Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails

SIMPLE SUMMARY: It is important to decide what to eat and what not to eat in the life. Children are likely to reject new foods. When eating a new food results in a negative experience, the child will avoid that specific food in the future. This phenomenon is called ‘conditioned taste aversion’ in ma...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Nakai, Junko, Totani, Yuki, Hatakeyama, Dai, Dyakonova, Varvara E., Ito, Etsuro
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: MDPI 2020
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7760351/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33256267
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology9120422
_version_ 1783627312409870336
author Nakai, Junko
Totani, Yuki
Hatakeyama, Dai
Dyakonova, Varvara E.
Ito, Etsuro
author_facet Nakai, Junko
Totani, Yuki
Hatakeyama, Dai
Dyakonova, Varvara E.
Ito, Etsuro
author_sort Nakai, Junko
collection PubMed
description SIMPLE SUMMARY: It is important to decide what to eat and what not to eat in the life. Children are likely to reject new foods. When eating a new food results in a negative experience, the child will avoid that specific food in the future. This phenomenon is called ‘conditioned taste aversion’ in mammals, and it is considered necessary for survival by preventing subsequent ingestion of sickening foods. Many researchers study the same kind of phenomenon in invertebrates, too. For example, the formation of conditioned taste aversion was found in the pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, with the selective associability between a sweet sucrose solution and a bitter KCl solution. A sweet food attracts many kinds of animals, resulting in the feeding response, whereas a KCl solution is an aversive stimulus, inducing a withdrawal response in snails. After repeated temporally-contingent presentations of these two stimuli, the sucrose solution no longer elicits a feeding response, and this phenomenon persists for a long term. In the present review, we first outline the mechanisms of conditioned taste aversion in mammals, then introduce the conditioned taste aversion in snails, and compare them. Furthermore, the molecular events in snails are discussed, suggesting the general mechanism in conditioned taste aversion. ABSTRACT: Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in mammals has several specific characteristics: (1) emergence of a negative symptom in subjects due to selective association with a taste-related stimulus, (2) robust long-term memory that is resistant to extinction induced by repeated presentation of the conditioned stimulus (CS), (3) a very-long-delay presentation of the unconditioned stimulus (US), and (4) single-trial learning. The pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, can also form a CTA. Although the negative symptoms, like nausea, in humans cannot be easily observed in invertebrate animal models of CTA, all the other characteristics of CTA seem to be present in snails. Selective associability was confirmed using a sweet sucrose solution and a bitter KCl solution. Once snails form a CTA, repeated presentation of the CS does not extinguish the CTA. A long interstimulus interval between the CS and US, like in trace conditioning, still results in the formation of a CTA in snails. Lastly, even single-trial learning has been demonstrated with a certain probability. In the present review, we compare, in detail, CTA in mammals and snails, and discuss the possible molecular events in CTA.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7760351
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2020
publisher MDPI
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-77603512020-12-26 Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails Nakai, Junko Totani, Yuki Hatakeyama, Dai Dyakonova, Varvara E. Ito, Etsuro Biology (Basel) Review SIMPLE SUMMARY: It is important to decide what to eat and what not to eat in the life. Children are likely to reject new foods. When eating a new food results in a negative experience, the child will avoid that specific food in the future. This phenomenon is called ‘conditioned taste aversion’ in mammals, and it is considered necessary for survival by preventing subsequent ingestion of sickening foods. Many researchers study the same kind of phenomenon in invertebrates, too. For example, the formation of conditioned taste aversion was found in the pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, with the selective associability between a sweet sucrose solution and a bitter KCl solution. A sweet food attracts many kinds of animals, resulting in the feeding response, whereas a KCl solution is an aversive stimulus, inducing a withdrawal response in snails. After repeated temporally-contingent presentations of these two stimuli, the sucrose solution no longer elicits a feeding response, and this phenomenon persists for a long term. In the present review, we first outline the mechanisms of conditioned taste aversion in mammals, then introduce the conditioned taste aversion in snails, and compare them. Furthermore, the molecular events in snails are discussed, suggesting the general mechanism in conditioned taste aversion. ABSTRACT: Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in mammals has several specific characteristics: (1) emergence of a negative symptom in subjects due to selective association with a taste-related stimulus, (2) robust long-term memory that is resistant to extinction induced by repeated presentation of the conditioned stimulus (CS), (3) a very-long-delay presentation of the unconditioned stimulus (US), and (4) single-trial learning. The pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, can also form a CTA. Although the negative symptoms, like nausea, in humans cannot be easily observed in invertebrate animal models of CTA, all the other characteristics of CTA seem to be present in snails. Selective associability was confirmed using a sweet sucrose solution and a bitter KCl solution. Once snails form a CTA, repeated presentation of the CS does not extinguish the CTA. A long interstimulus interval between the CS and US, like in trace conditioning, still results in the formation of a CTA in snails. Lastly, even single-trial learning has been demonstrated with a certain probability. In the present review, we compare, in detail, CTA in mammals and snails, and discuss the possible molecular events in CTA. MDPI 2020-11-26 /pmc/articles/PMC7760351/ /pubmed/33256267 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology9120422 Text en © 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Review
Nakai, Junko
Totani, Yuki
Hatakeyama, Dai
Dyakonova, Varvara E.
Ito, Etsuro
Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title_full Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title_fullStr Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title_full_unstemmed Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title_short Another Example of Conditioned Taste Aversion: Case of Snails
title_sort another example of conditioned taste aversion: case of snails
topic Review
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7760351/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33256267
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology9120422
work_keys_str_mv AT nakaijunko anotherexampleofconditionedtasteaversioncaseofsnails
AT totaniyuki anotherexampleofconditionedtasteaversioncaseofsnails
AT hatakeyamadai anotherexampleofconditionedtasteaversioncaseofsnails
AT dyakonovavarvarae anotherexampleofconditionedtasteaversioncaseofsnails
AT itoetsuro anotherexampleofconditionedtasteaversioncaseofsnails