Cargando…

Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task

While aiming and shooting, we make tiny eye movements called microsaccades that shift gaze between task-relevant objects within a small region of the visual field. However, in the brief period before pressing the trigger, microsaccades are suppressed. This might be due to the lack of a requirement t...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Nanjappa, Rakesh, McPeek, Robert M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7884291/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33570557
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.2.6
_version_ 1783651381753675776
author Nanjappa, Rakesh
McPeek, Robert M.
author_facet Nanjappa, Rakesh
McPeek, Robert M.
author_sort Nanjappa, Rakesh
collection PubMed
description While aiming and shooting, we make tiny eye movements called microsaccades that shift gaze between task-relevant objects within a small region of the visual field. However, in the brief period before pressing the trigger, microsaccades are suppressed. This might be due to the lack of a requirement to shift gaze as the retinal images of the two objects begin to overlap on the fovea. Alternatively, we might actively suppress microsaccades to prevent any disturbances in visual perception caused by microsaccades around the time of their occurrence and their subsequent effect on shooting performance. In this study we looked at microsaccade rates while participants performed a simulated shooting task under two conditions: a normal condition in which they moved their eyes freely, and an eccentric condition in which they maintained gaze on a fixed target while performing the shooting task at 5° eccentricity. As expected, microsaccade rate dropped near the end of the task in the normal viewing condition. However, we also found the same decrease for the eccentric condition in which microsaccades did not shift gaze between the task objects. Microsaccades are also produced in response to shifts in covert attention. To test whether disengagement of covert attention from the eccentric shooting location caused the drop in microsaccade rate, we monitored the location of participants’ spatial attention by using a Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) task simultaneously at a location opposite to the shooting task. Target letter detection at the RSVP location did not improve during the drop in microsaccade rate, suggesting that covert attention was maintained at the shooting task location. We conclude that in addition to their usual gaze-shifting function, microsaccades during fine-acuity tasks might be modulated by cognitive processes other than spatial attention.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-7884291
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-78842912021-02-22 Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task Nanjappa, Rakesh McPeek, Robert M. J Vis Article While aiming and shooting, we make tiny eye movements called microsaccades that shift gaze between task-relevant objects within a small region of the visual field. However, in the brief period before pressing the trigger, microsaccades are suppressed. This might be due to the lack of a requirement to shift gaze as the retinal images of the two objects begin to overlap on the fovea. Alternatively, we might actively suppress microsaccades to prevent any disturbances in visual perception caused by microsaccades around the time of their occurrence and their subsequent effect on shooting performance. In this study we looked at microsaccade rates while participants performed a simulated shooting task under two conditions: a normal condition in which they moved their eyes freely, and an eccentric condition in which they maintained gaze on a fixed target while performing the shooting task at 5° eccentricity. As expected, microsaccade rate dropped near the end of the task in the normal viewing condition. However, we also found the same decrease for the eccentric condition in which microsaccades did not shift gaze between the task objects. Microsaccades are also produced in response to shifts in covert attention. To test whether disengagement of covert attention from the eccentric shooting location caused the drop in microsaccade rate, we monitored the location of participants’ spatial attention by using a Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) task simultaneously at a location opposite to the shooting task. Target letter detection at the RSVP location did not improve during the drop in microsaccade rate, suggesting that covert attention was maintained at the shooting task location. We conclude that in addition to their usual gaze-shifting function, microsaccades during fine-acuity tasks might be modulated by cognitive processes other than spatial attention. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2021-02-11 /pmc/articles/PMC7884291/ /pubmed/33570557 http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.2.6 Text en Copyright 2021 The Authors http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
spellingShingle Article
Nanjappa, Rakesh
McPeek, Robert M.
Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title_full Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title_fullStr Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title_full_unstemmed Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title_short Microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
title_sort microsaccades and attention in a high-acuity visual alignment task
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7884291/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33570557
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.2.6
work_keys_str_mv AT nanjapparakesh microsaccadesandattentioninahighacuityvisualalignmenttask
AT mcpeekrobertm microsaccadesandattentioninahighacuityvisualalignmenttask