Cargando…
Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge
The question of when children understand that others have minds that can represent or misrepresent reality (i.e., possess a ‘Theory of Mind’) is hotly debated. This understanding plays a fundamental role in social interaction (e.g., interpreting human behavior, communicating, empathizing). Most rese...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7894954/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33606742 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244141 |
_version_ | 1783653335814897664 |
---|---|
author | Ghrear, Siba Baimel, Adam Haddock, Taeh Birch, Susan A. J. |
author_facet | Ghrear, Siba Baimel, Adam Haddock, Taeh Birch, Susan A. J. |
author_sort | Ghrear, Siba |
collection | PubMed |
description | The question of when children understand that others have minds that can represent or misrepresent reality (i.e., possess a ‘Theory of Mind’) is hotly debated. This understanding plays a fundamental role in social interaction (e.g., interpreting human behavior, communicating, empathizing). Most research on this topic has relied on false belief tasks such as the ‘Sally-Anne Task’, because researchers have argued that it is the strongest litmus test examining one’s understanding that the mind can misrepresent reality. Unfortunately, in addition to a variety of other cognitive demands this widely used measure also unnecessarily involves overcoming a bias that is especially pronounced in young children—the ‘curse of knowledge’ (the tendency to be biased by one’s knowledge when considering less-informed perspectives). Three- to 6-year-old’s (n = 230) false belief reasoning was examined across tasks that either did, or did not, require overcoming the curse of knowledge, revealing that when the curse of knowledge was removed three-year-olds were significantly better at inferring false beliefs, and as accurate as five- and six-year-olds. These findings reveal that the classic task is not specifically measuring false belief understanding. Instead, previously observed developmental changes in children’s performance could be attributed to the ability to overcome the curse of knowledge. Similarly, previously observed relationships between individual differences in false belief reasoning and a variety of social outcomes could instead be the result of individual differences in the ability to overcome the curse of knowledge, highlighting the need to re-evaluate how best to interpret large bodies of research on false belief reasoning and social-emotional functioning. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-7894954 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-78949542021-03-01 Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge Ghrear, Siba Baimel, Adam Haddock, Taeh Birch, Susan A. J. PLoS One Research Article The question of when children understand that others have minds that can represent or misrepresent reality (i.e., possess a ‘Theory of Mind’) is hotly debated. This understanding plays a fundamental role in social interaction (e.g., interpreting human behavior, communicating, empathizing). Most research on this topic has relied on false belief tasks such as the ‘Sally-Anne Task’, because researchers have argued that it is the strongest litmus test examining one’s understanding that the mind can misrepresent reality. Unfortunately, in addition to a variety of other cognitive demands this widely used measure also unnecessarily involves overcoming a bias that is especially pronounced in young children—the ‘curse of knowledge’ (the tendency to be biased by one’s knowledge when considering less-informed perspectives). Three- to 6-year-old’s (n = 230) false belief reasoning was examined across tasks that either did, or did not, require overcoming the curse of knowledge, revealing that when the curse of knowledge was removed three-year-olds were significantly better at inferring false beliefs, and as accurate as five- and six-year-olds. These findings reveal that the classic task is not specifically measuring false belief understanding. Instead, previously observed developmental changes in children’s performance could be attributed to the ability to overcome the curse of knowledge. Similarly, previously observed relationships between individual differences in false belief reasoning and a variety of social outcomes could instead be the result of individual differences in the ability to overcome the curse of knowledge, highlighting the need to re-evaluate how best to interpret large bodies of research on false belief reasoning and social-emotional functioning. Public Library of Science 2021-02-19 /pmc/articles/PMC7894954/ /pubmed/33606742 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244141 Text en © 2021 Ghrear et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Ghrear, Siba Baimel, Adam Haddock, Taeh Birch, Susan A. J. Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title | Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title_full | Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title_fullStr | Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title_full_unstemmed | Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title_short | Are the classic false belief tasks cursed? Young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
title_sort | are the classic false belief tasks cursed? young children are just as likely as older children to pass a false belief task when they are not required to overcome the curse of knowledge |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7894954/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33606742 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244141 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT ghrearsiba aretheclassicfalsebelieftaskscursedyoungchildrenarejustaslikelyasolderchildrentopassafalsebelieftaskwhentheyarenotrequiredtoovercomethecurseofknowledge AT baimeladam aretheclassicfalsebelieftaskscursedyoungchildrenarejustaslikelyasolderchildrentopassafalsebelieftaskwhentheyarenotrequiredtoovercomethecurseofknowledge AT haddocktaeh aretheclassicfalsebelieftaskscursedyoungchildrenarejustaslikelyasolderchildrentopassafalsebelieftaskwhentheyarenotrequiredtoovercomethecurseofknowledge AT birchsusanaj aretheclassicfalsebelieftaskscursedyoungchildrenarejustaslikelyasolderchildrentopassafalsebelieftaskwhentheyarenotrequiredtoovercomethecurseofknowledge |