Cargando…
Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8061313/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33897560 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889 |
_version_ | 1783681540580966400 |
---|---|
author | Foinant, Damien Lafraire, Jérémie Thibaut, Jean-Pierre |
author_facet | Foinant, Damien Lafraire, Jérémie Thibaut, Jean-Pierre |
author_sort | Foinant, Damien |
collection | PubMed |
description | Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to a normal and healthy development. To face this challenge, it is essential to investigate how children generalize these positive and negative properties to other foods, including familiar and unfamiliar ones. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that children might rely on cues of food processing (e.g., signs of human intervention such as slicing) to convey information about item edibility. Furthermore, capitalizing on previous results showing that food rejections (i.e., food neophobia and picky eating) are a significant source of inter-individual variability to children’s inferences in the food domain, we followed an individual approach. We expected that children would generalize the positive properties to familiar foods and, in contrast, that they would generalize more often the negative properties to unfamiliar foods. However, we expected that children would generalize more positive and less negative properties to unfamiliar sliced foods than to whole unfamiliar foods. Finally, we expected that children displaying higher levels of food rejections would generalize more negative properties than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. One-hundred and twenty-six children, aged 3–6 years, performed an induction task in which they had to generalize positive or negative health-related properties to familiar or unfamiliar foods, whole or sliced. We measured children’s probability of generalization for positive and negative properties. The children’s food rejection score was assessed on a standardized scale. Results indicated that children evaluated positively familiar foods (regardless of processing), whereas they tend to view unfamiliar food negatively. In contrast, children were at chance for processed unfamiliar foods. Furthermore, children displaying higher levels of food rejections were more likely to generalize the negative properties to all kinds of foods than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. These findings entitle us to hypothesize that knowledge-based food education programs should take into account the valence of the properties taught to children, as well as the state of processing of the food presented. Furthermore, one should take children’s interindividual differences into account because they influence how the knowledge gained through these programs may be generalized. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8061313 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-80613132021-04-23 Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption Foinant, Damien Lafraire, Jérémie Thibaut, Jean-Pierre Front Psychol Psychology Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to a normal and healthy development. To face this challenge, it is essential to investigate how children generalize these positive and negative properties to other foods, including familiar and unfamiliar ones. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that children might rely on cues of food processing (e.g., signs of human intervention such as slicing) to convey information about item edibility. Furthermore, capitalizing on previous results showing that food rejections (i.e., food neophobia and picky eating) are a significant source of inter-individual variability to children’s inferences in the food domain, we followed an individual approach. We expected that children would generalize the positive properties to familiar foods and, in contrast, that they would generalize more often the negative properties to unfamiliar foods. However, we expected that children would generalize more positive and less negative properties to unfamiliar sliced foods than to whole unfamiliar foods. Finally, we expected that children displaying higher levels of food rejections would generalize more negative properties than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. One-hundred and twenty-six children, aged 3–6 years, performed an induction task in which they had to generalize positive or negative health-related properties to familiar or unfamiliar foods, whole or sliced. We measured children’s probability of generalization for positive and negative properties. The children’s food rejection score was assessed on a standardized scale. Results indicated that children evaluated positively familiar foods (regardless of processing), whereas they tend to view unfamiliar food negatively. In contrast, children were at chance for processed unfamiliar foods. Furthermore, children displaying higher levels of food rejections were more likely to generalize the negative properties to all kinds of foods than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. These findings entitle us to hypothesize that knowledge-based food education programs should take into account the valence of the properties taught to children, as well as the state of processing of the food presented. Furthermore, one should take children’s interindividual differences into account because they influence how the knowledge gained through these programs may be generalized. Frontiers Media S.A. 2021-04-08 /pmc/articles/PMC8061313/ /pubmed/33897560 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889 Text en Copyright © 2021 Foinant, Lafraire and Thibaut. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Foinant, Damien Lafraire, Jérémie Thibaut, Jean-Pierre Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title | Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title_full | Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title_fullStr | Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title_full_unstemmed | Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title_short | Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption |
title_sort | strength or nausea? children’s reasoning about the health consequences of food consumption |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8061313/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33897560 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT foinantdamien strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption AT lafrairejeremie strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption AT thibautjeanpierre strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption |