Cargando…

Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control

Cognitive control processes encompass many distinct components, including response inhibition (stopping a prepotent response), proactive control (using prior information to enact control), reactive control (last-minute changing of a prepotent response), and conflict monitoring (choosing between two...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Eisma, Jarrod, Rawls, Eric, Long, Stephanie, Mach, Russell, Lamm, Connie
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Nature Publishing Group UK 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8290013/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34282209
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94162-z
_version_ 1783724404617773056
author Eisma, Jarrod
Rawls, Eric
Long, Stephanie
Mach, Russell
Lamm, Connie
author_facet Eisma, Jarrod
Rawls, Eric
Long, Stephanie
Mach, Russell
Lamm, Connie
author_sort Eisma, Jarrod
collection PubMed
description Cognitive control processes encompass many distinct components, including response inhibition (stopping a prepotent response), proactive control (using prior information to enact control), reactive control (last-minute changing of a prepotent response), and conflict monitoring (choosing between two competing responses). While frontal midline theta activity is theorized to be a general marker of the need for cognitive control, a stringent test of this hypothesis would require a quantitative, within-subject comparison of the neural activation patterns indexing many different cognitive control strategies, an experiment lacking in the current literature. We recorded EEG from 176 participants as they performed tasks that tested inhibitory control (Go/Nogo Task), proactive and reactive control (AX-Continuous Performance Task), and resolving response conflict (Global/Local Task-modified Flanker Task). As activity in the theta (4–8 Hz) frequency band is thought to be a common signature of cognitive control, we assessed frontal midline theta activation underlying each cognitive control strategy. In all strategies, we found higher frontal midline theta power for trials that required more cognitive control (target conditions) versus control conditions. Additionally, reactive control and inhibitory control had higher theta power than proactive control and response conflict, and proactive control had higher theta power than response conflict. Using decoding analyses, we were able to successfully decode control from target trials using classifiers trained exclusively on each of the other strategies, thus firmly demonstrating that theta representations of cognitive control generalize across multiple cognitive control strategies. Our results confirm that frontal midline theta-band activity is a common mechanism for initiating and executing cognitive control, but theta power also differentiates between cognitive control mechanisms. As theta activation reliably differs depending on the cognitive control strategy employed, future work will need to focus on the differential role of theta in differing cognitive control strategies.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8290013
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher Nature Publishing Group UK
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-82900132021-07-21 Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control Eisma, Jarrod Rawls, Eric Long, Stephanie Mach, Russell Lamm, Connie Sci Rep Article Cognitive control processes encompass many distinct components, including response inhibition (stopping a prepotent response), proactive control (using prior information to enact control), reactive control (last-minute changing of a prepotent response), and conflict monitoring (choosing between two competing responses). While frontal midline theta activity is theorized to be a general marker of the need for cognitive control, a stringent test of this hypothesis would require a quantitative, within-subject comparison of the neural activation patterns indexing many different cognitive control strategies, an experiment lacking in the current literature. We recorded EEG from 176 participants as they performed tasks that tested inhibitory control (Go/Nogo Task), proactive and reactive control (AX-Continuous Performance Task), and resolving response conflict (Global/Local Task-modified Flanker Task). As activity in the theta (4–8 Hz) frequency band is thought to be a common signature of cognitive control, we assessed frontal midline theta activation underlying each cognitive control strategy. In all strategies, we found higher frontal midline theta power for trials that required more cognitive control (target conditions) versus control conditions. Additionally, reactive control and inhibitory control had higher theta power than proactive control and response conflict, and proactive control had higher theta power than response conflict. Using decoding analyses, we were able to successfully decode control from target trials using classifiers trained exclusively on each of the other strategies, thus firmly demonstrating that theta representations of cognitive control generalize across multiple cognitive control strategies. Our results confirm that frontal midline theta-band activity is a common mechanism for initiating and executing cognitive control, but theta power also differentiates between cognitive control mechanisms. As theta activation reliably differs depending on the cognitive control strategy employed, future work will need to focus on the differential role of theta in differing cognitive control strategies. Nature Publishing Group UK 2021-07-19 /pmc/articles/PMC8290013/ /pubmed/34282209 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94162-z Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Article
Eisma, Jarrod
Rawls, Eric
Long, Stephanie
Mach, Russell
Lamm, Connie
Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title_full Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title_fullStr Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title_full_unstemmed Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title_short Frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
title_sort frontal midline theta differentiates separate cognitive control strategies while still generalizing the need for cognitive control
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8290013/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34282209
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94162-z
work_keys_str_mv AT eismajarrod frontalmidlinethetadifferentiatesseparatecognitivecontrolstrategieswhilestillgeneralizingtheneedforcognitivecontrol
AT rawlseric frontalmidlinethetadifferentiatesseparatecognitivecontrolstrategieswhilestillgeneralizingtheneedforcognitivecontrol
AT longstephanie frontalmidlinethetadifferentiatesseparatecognitivecontrolstrategieswhilestillgeneralizingtheneedforcognitivecontrol
AT machrussell frontalmidlinethetadifferentiatesseparatecognitivecontrolstrategieswhilestillgeneralizingtheneedforcognitivecontrol
AT lammconnie frontalmidlinethetadifferentiatesseparatecognitivecontrolstrategieswhilestillgeneralizingtheneedforcognitivecontrol