Cargando…

Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds

Rhythm perception is fundamental to speech and music. Humans readily recognize a rhythmic pattern, such as that of a familiar song, independently of the tempo at which it occurs. This shows that our perception of auditory rhythms is flexible, relying on global relational patterns more than on the ab...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Rouse, Andrew A., Patel, Aniruddh D., Kao, Mimi H.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: National Academy of Sciences 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8307534/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34272278
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2026130118
_version_ 1783728072216805376
author Rouse, Andrew A.
Patel, Aniruddh D.
Kao, Mimi H.
author_facet Rouse, Andrew A.
Patel, Aniruddh D.
Kao, Mimi H.
author_sort Rouse, Andrew A.
collection PubMed
description Rhythm perception is fundamental to speech and music. Humans readily recognize a rhythmic pattern, such as that of a familiar song, independently of the tempo at which it occurs. This shows that our perception of auditory rhythms is flexible, relying on global relational patterns more than on the absolute durations of specific time intervals. Given that auditory rhythm perception in humans engages a complex auditory–motor cortical network even in the absence of movement and that the evolution of vocal learning is accompanied by strengthening of forebrain auditory–motor pathways, we hypothesize that vocal learning species share our perceptual facility for relational rhythm processing. We test this by asking whether the best-studied animal model for vocal learning, the zebra finch, can recognize a fundamental rhythmic pattern—equal timing between event onsets (isochrony)—based on temporal relations between intervals rather than on absolute durations. Prior work suggests that vocal nonlearners (pigeons and rats) are quite limited in this regard and are biased to attend to absolute durations when listening to rhythmic sequences. In contrast, using naturalistic sounds at multiple stimulus rates, we show that male zebra finches robustly recognize isochrony independent of absolute time intervals, even at rates distant from those used in training. Our findings highlight the importance of comparative studies of rhythmic processing and suggest that vocal learning species are promising animal models for key aspects of human rhythm perception. Such models are needed to understand the neural mechanisms behind the positive effect of rhythm on certain speech and movement disorders.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8307534
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher National Academy of Sciences
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-83075342021-07-28 Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds Rouse, Andrew A. Patel, Aniruddh D. Kao, Mimi H. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Biological Sciences Rhythm perception is fundamental to speech and music. Humans readily recognize a rhythmic pattern, such as that of a familiar song, independently of the tempo at which it occurs. This shows that our perception of auditory rhythms is flexible, relying on global relational patterns more than on the absolute durations of specific time intervals. Given that auditory rhythm perception in humans engages a complex auditory–motor cortical network even in the absence of movement and that the evolution of vocal learning is accompanied by strengthening of forebrain auditory–motor pathways, we hypothesize that vocal learning species share our perceptual facility for relational rhythm processing. We test this by asking whether the best-studied animal model for vocal learning, the zebra finch, can recognize a fundamental rhythmic pattern—equal timing between event onsets (isochrony)—based on temporal relations between intervals rather than on absolute durations. Prior work suggests that vocal nonlearners (pigeons and rats) are quite limited in this regard and are biased to attend to absolute durations when listening to rhythmic sequences. In contrast, using naturalistic sounds at multiple stimulus rates, we show that male zebra finches robustly recognize isochrony independent of absolute time intervals, even at rates distant from those used in training. Our findings highlight the importance of comparative studies of rhythmic processing and suggest that vocal learning species are promising animal models for key aspects of human rhythm perception. Such models are needed to understand the neural mechanisms behind the positive effect of rhythm on certain speech and movement disorders. National Academy of Sciences 2021-07-20 2021-07-16 /pmc/articles/PMC8307534/ /pubmed/34272278 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2026130118 Text en Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Biological Sciences
Rouse, Andrew A.
Patel, Aniruddh D.
Kao, Mimi H.
Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title_full Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title_fullStr Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title_full_unstemmed Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title_short Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: Evidence from songbirds
title_sort vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: evidence from songbirds
topic Biological Sciences
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8307534/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34272278
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2026130118
work_keys_str_mv AT rouseandrewa vocallearningandflexiblerhythmpatternperceptionarelinkedevidencefromsongbirds
AT patelaniruddhd vocallearningandflexiblerhythmpatternperceptionarelinkedevidencefromsongbirds
AT kaomimih vocallearningandflexiblerhythmpatternperceptionarelinkedevidencefromsongbirds