Cargando…

Magnitude integration in the Archerfish

We make magnitude-related decisions every day, for example, to choose the shortest queue at the grocery store. When making such decisions, which magnitudes do we consider? The dominant theory suggests that our focus is on numerical quantity, i.e., the number of items in a set. This theory leads to q...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Leibovich-Raveh, Tali, Raveh, Ashael, Vilker, Dana, Gabay, Shai
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Nature Publishing Group UK 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8329031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34341367
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94956-1
_version_ 1783732411389968384
author Leibovich-Raveh, Tali
Raveh, Ashael
Vilker, Dana
Gabay, Shai
author_facet Leibovich-Raveh, Tali
Raveh, Ashael
Vilker, Dana
Gabay, Shai
author_sort Leibovich-Raveh, Tali
collection PubMed
description We make magnitude-related decisions every day, for example, to choose the shortest queue at the grocery store. When making such decisions, which magnitudes do we consider? The dominant theory suggests that our focus is on numerical quantity, i.e., the number of items in a set. This theory leads to quantity-focused research suggesting that discriminating quantities is automatic, innate, and is the basis for mathematical abilities in humans. Another theory suggests, instead, that non-numerical magnitudes, such as the total area of the compared items, are usually what humans rely on, and numerical quantity is used only when required. Since wild animals must make quick magnitude-related decisions to eat, seek shelter, survive, and procreate, studying which magnitudes animals spontaneously use in magnitude-related decisions is a good way to study the relative primacy of numerical quantity versus non-numerical magnitudes. We asked whether, in an animal model, the influence of non-numerical magnitudes on performance in a spontaneous magnitude comparison task is modulated by the number of non-numerical magnitudes that positively correlate with numerical quantity. Our animal model was the Archerfish, a fish that, in the wild, hunts insects by shooting a jet of water at them. These fish were trained to shoot water at artificial targets presented on a computer screen above the water tank. We tested the Archerfish's performance in spontaneous, untrained two-choice magnitude decisions. We found that the fish tended to select the group containing larger non-numerical magnitudes and smaller quantities of dots. The fish selected the group containing more dots mostly when the quantity of the dots was positively correlated with all five different non-numerical magnitudes. The current study adds to the body of studies providing direct evidence that in some cases animals’ magnitude-related decisions are more affected by non-numerical magnitudes than by numerical quantity, putting doubt on the claims that numerical quantity perception is the most basic building block of mathematical abilities.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8329031
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher Nature Publishing Group UK
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-83290312021-08-03 Magnitude integration in the Archerfish Leibovich-Raveh, Tali Raveh, Ashael Vilker, Dana Gabay, Shai Sci Rep Article We make magnitude-related decisions every day, for example, to choose the shortest queue at the grocery store. When making such decisions, which magnitudes do we consider? The dominant theory suggests that our focus is on numerical quantity, i.e., the number of items in a set. This theory leads to quantity-focused research suggesting that discriminating quantities is automatic, innate, and is the basis for mathematical abilities in humans. Another theory suggests, instead, that non-numerical magnitudes, such as the total area of the compared items, are usually what humans rely on, and numerical quantity is used only when required. Since wild animals must make quick magnitude-related decisions to eat, seek shelter, survive, and procreate, studying which magnitudes animals spontaneously use in magnitude-related decisions is a good way to study the relative primacy of numerical quantity versus non-numerical magnitudes. We asked whether, in an animal model, the influence of non-numerical magnitudes on performance in a spontaneous magnitude comparison task is modulated by the number of non-numerical magnitudes that positively correlate with numerical quantity. Our animal model was the Archerfish, a fish that, in the wild, hunts insects by shooting a jet of water at them. These fish were trained to shoot water at artificial targets presented on a computer screen above the water tank. We tested the Archerfish's performance in spontaneous, untrained two-choice magnitude decisions. We found that the fish tended to select the group containing larger non-numerical magnitudes and smaller quantities of dots. The fish selected the group containing more dots mostly when the quantity of the dots was positively correlated with all five different non-numerical magnitudes. The current study adds to the body of studies providing direct evidence that in some cases animals’ magnitude-related decisions are more affected by non-numerical magnitudes than by numerical quantity, putting doubt on the claims that numerical quantity perception is the most basic building block of mathematical abilities. Nature Publishing Group UK 2021-08-02 /pmc/articles/PMC8329031/ /pubmed/34341367 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94956-1 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Article
Leibovich-Raveh, Tali
Raveh, Ashael
Vilker, Dana
Gabay, Shai
Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title_full Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title_fullStr Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title_full_unstemmed Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title_short Magnitude integration in the Archerfish
title_sort magnitude integration in the archerfish
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8329031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34341367
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-94956-1
work_keys_str_mv AT leibovichravehtali magnitudeintegrationinthearcherfish
AT ravehashael magnitudeintegrationinthearcherfish
AT vilkerdana magnitudeintegrationinthearcherfish
AT gabayshai magnitudeintegrationinthearcherfish