Cargando…

Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage

That binocular viewing confers an advantage over monocular viewing for detecting isolated low luminance or low contrast objects, has been known for well over a century; however, the processes involved in combining the images from the two eyes are still not fully understood. Importantly, in natural v...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Lev, Maria, Ding, Jian, Polat, Uri, Levi, Dennis M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Nature Publishing Group UK 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8376993/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34413354
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-96053-9
_version_ 1783740567001235456
author Lev, Maria
Ding, Jian
Polat, Uri
Levi, Dennis M.
author_facet Lev, Maria
Ding, Jian
Polat, Uri
Levi, Dennis M.
author_sort Lev, Maria
collection PubMed
description That binocular viewing confers an advantage over monocular viewing for detecting isolated low luminance or low contrast objects, has been known for well over a century; however, the processes involved in combining the images from the two eyes are still not fully understood. Importantly, in natural vision, objects are rarely isolated but appear in context. It is well known that nearby contours can either facilitate or suppress detection, depending on their distance from the target and the global configuration. Here we report that at close distances collinear (but not orthogonal) flanking contours suppress detection more under binocular compared to monocular viewing, thus completely abolishing the binocular advantage, both at threshold and suprathreshold levels. In contrast, more distant flankers facilitate both monocular and binocular detection, preserving a binocular advantage up to about four times the detection threshold. Our results for monocular and binocular viewing, for threshold contrast discrimination without nearby flankers, can be explained by a gain control model with uncertainty and internal multiplicative noise adding additional constraints on detection. However, in context with nearby flankers, both contrast detection threshold and suprathreshold contrast appearance matching require the addition of both target-to-target and flank-to-target interactions occurring before the site of binocular combination. To test an alternative model, in which the interactions occur after the site of binocular combination, we performed a dichoptic contrast matching experiment, with the target presented to one eye, and the flanks to the other eye. The two models make very different predictions for abutting flanks under dichoptic conditions. Interactions after the combination site predict that the perceived contrast of the flanked target will be strongly suppressed, while interactions before the site predict the perceived contrast will be more or less veridical. The data are consistent with the latter model, strongly suggesting that the interactions take place before the site of binocular combination.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8376993
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher Nature Publishing Group UK
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-83769932021-08-27 Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage Lev, Maria Ding, Jian Polat, Uri Levi, Dennis M. Sci Rep Article That binocular viewing confers an advantage over monocular viewing for detecting isolated low luminance or low contrast objects, has been known for well over a century; however, the processes involved in combining the images from the two eyes are still not fully understood. Importantly, in natural vision, objects are rarely isolated but appear in context. It is well known that nearby contours can either facilitate or suppress detection, depending on their distance from the target and the global configuration. Here we report that at close distances collinear (but not orthogonal) flanking contours suppress detection more under binocular compared to monocular viewing, thus completely abolishing the binocular advantage, both at threshold and suprathreshold levels. In contrast, more distant flankers facilitate both monocular and binocular detection, preserving a binocular advantage up to about four times the detection threshold. Our results for monocular and binocular viewing, for threshold contrast discrimination without nearby flankers, can be explained by a gain control model with uncertainty and internal multiplicative noise adding additional constraints on detection. However, in context with nearby flankers, both contrast detection threshold and suprathreshold contrast appearance matching require the addition of both target-to-target and flank-to-target interactions occurring before the site of binocular combination. To test an alternative model, in which the interactions occur after the site of binocular combination, we performed a dichoptic contrast matching experiment, with the target presented to one eye, and the flanks to the other eye. The two models make very different predictions for abutting flanks under dichoptic conditions. Interactions after the combination site predict that the perceived contrast of the flanked target will be strongly suppressed, while interactions before the site predict the perceived contrast will be more or less veridical. The data are consistent with the latter model, strongly suggesting that the interactions take place before the site of binocular combination. Nature Publishing Group UK 2021-08-19 /pmc/articles/PMC8376993/ /pubmed/34413354 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-96053-9 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Article
Lev, Maria
Ding, Jian
Polat, Uri
Levi, Dennis M.
Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title_full Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title_fullStr Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title_full_unstemmed Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title_short Nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
title_sort nearby contours abolish the binocular advantage
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8376993/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34413354
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-96053-9
work_keys_str_mv AT levmaria nearbycontoursabolishthebinocularadvantage
AT dingjian nearbycontoursabolishthebinocularadvantage
AT polaturi nearbycontoursabolishthebinocularadvantage
AT levidennism nearbycontoursabolishthebinocularadvantage