Cargando…
Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia
OBJECTIVES: Paranoia is known to be associated with insecure attachment, with negative self‐esteem as a mediator, but this pathway is insufficient to explain the paranoid individual’s beliefs about malevolent others. Mistrust is a likely additional factor as it is a core feature of paranoid thinking...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
John Wiley and Sons Inc.
2020
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8451824/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33314565 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/papt.12314 |
_version_ | 1784569932469501952 |
---|---|
author | Martinez, Anton P. Agostini, Maximilian Al‐Suhibani, Azzam Bentall, Richard P. |
author_facet | Martinez, Anton P. Agostini, Maximilian Al‐Suhibani, Azzam Bentall, Richard P. |
author_sort | Martinez, Anton P. |
collection | PubMed |
description | OBJECTIVES: Paranoia is known to be associated with insecure attachment, with negative self‐esteem as a mediator, but this pathway is insufficient to explain the paranoid individual’s beliefs about malevolent others. Mistrust is a likely additional factor as it is a core feature of paranoid thinking also associated with insecure attachment styles. In this study, we tested whether mistrust – operationalized as judgements about the trustworthiness of unfamiliar faces – constitutes a second pathway from insecure attachment to paranoia. DESIGN: The design of the study was cross‐sectional. METHODS: A nationally representative British sample of 1,508 participants aged 18–86, 50.8% female, recruited through the survey company Qualtrics, completed measurements of attachment style, negative self‐esteem, and paranoid beliefs. Usable data were obtained from 1,121 participants. Participants were asked to make trustworthiness judgements about computer‐generated faces, and their outcomes were analysed by conducting signal detection analysis, which provided measures of bias (the tendency to assume untrustworthiness in conditions of uncertainty) and sensitivity (accuracy in distinguish between trustworthy and untrustworthy faces). RESULTS: Results using structural equation modelling revealed a good model fit (RMSEA = .071, 95% CI: 0.067–0.075, SRMR = .045, CFI = .93, TLI = .92). We observed indirect effects through bias towards mistrust both for the relationship between attachment anxiety and avoidance (β = .003, 95% CI: 0.001–0.005,p < .001) and attachment anxiety and paranoia (β = .003, 95% CI 0.002–0.006, p < .001). We observed an indirect effect through negative self‐esteem only for the relationship between attachment anxiety and paranoia (β = .064, 95% CI: 0.053–0.077, p < .001). Trust judgements and negative self‐esteem were not associated with each other. CONCLUSIONS: We find that a bias towards mistrust is associated with greater paranoia. We also find indirect effects through bias towards mistrust between attachment styles and paranoia. Finally, we reaffirm the strong indirect effect through negative self‐esteem between attachment anxiety and paranoia. Limitations of the study are discussed. PRACTITIONER POINTS: When working with individuals suffering from paranoia, clinicians should consider not only explicit, deliberative cognitive processes of the kind addressed in cognitive behaviour therapy (e.g. cognitive restructuring) but also the way in which their patients make perceptual judgements (e.g., their immediate reactions on encountering new people) and consider interventions targeted at these judgements, for example, bias modification training. Assessment and clinical interventions for people should consider the role of trust judgements and the way in which they combine with low self‐esteem to provoke paranoid beliefs. Psychological interventions targeting paranoid beliefs should focus on both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8451824 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2020 |
publisher | John Wiley and Sons Inc. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-84518242021-09-27 Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia Martinez, Anton P. Agostini, Maximilian Al‐Suhibani, Azzam Bentall, Richard P. Psychol Psychother Research Papers OBJECTIVES: Paranoia is known to be associated with insecure attachment, with negative self‐esteem as a mediator, but this pathway is insufficient to explain the paranoid individual’s beliefs about malevolent others. Mistrust is a likely additional factor as it is a core feature of paranoid thinking also associated with insecure attachment styles. In this study, we tested whether mistrust – operationalized as judgements about the trustworthiness of unfamiliar faces – constitutes a second pathway from insecure attachment to paranoia. DESIGN: The design of the study was cross‐sectional. METHODS: A nationally representative British sample of 1,508 participants aged 18–86, 50.8% female, recruited through the survey company Qualtrics, completed measurements of attachment style, negative self‐esteem, and paranoid beliefs. Usable data were obtained from 1,121 participants. Participants were asked to make trustworthiness judgements about computer‐generated faces, and their outcomes were analysed by conducting signal detection analysis, which provided measures of bias (the tendency to assume untrustworthiness in conditions of uncertainty) and sensitivity (accuracy in distinguish between trustworthy and untrustworthy faces). RESULTS: Results using structural equation modelling revealed a good model fit (RMSEA = .071, 95% CI: 0.067–0.075, SRMR = .045, CFI = .93, TLI = .92). We observed indirect effects through bias towards mistrust both for the relationship between attachment anxiety and avoidance (β = .003, 95% CI: 0.001–0.005,p < .001) and attachment anxiety and paranoia (β = .003, 95% CI 0.002–0.006, p < .001). We observed an indirect effect through negative self‐esteem only for the relationship between attachment anxiety and paranoia (β = .064, 95% CI: 0.053–0.077, p < .001). Trust judgements and negative self‐esteem were not associated with each other. CONCLUSIONS: We find that a bias towards mistrust is associated with greater paranoia. We also find indirect effects through bias towards mistrust between attachment styles and paranoia. Finally, we reaffirm the strong indirect effect through negative self‐esteem between attachment anxiety and paranoia. Limitations of the study are discussed. PRACTITIONER POINTS: When working with individuals suffering from paranoia, clinicians should consider not only explicit, deliberative cognitive processes of the kind addressed in cognitive behaviour therapy (e.g. cognitive restructuring) but also the way in which their patients make perceptual judgements (e.g., their immediate reactions on encountering new people) and consider interventions targeted at these judgements, for example, bias modification training. Assessment and clinical interventions for people should consider the role of trust judgements and the way in which they combine with low self‐esteem to provoke paranoid beliefs. Psychological interventions targeting paranoid beliefs should focus on both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2020-12-13 2021-09 /pmc/articles/PMC8451824/ /pubmed/33314565 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/papt.12314 Text en © 2020 The Authors. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Psychological Society https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
spellingShingle | Research Papers Martinez, Anton P. Agostini, Maximilian Al‐Suhibani, Azzam Bentall, Richard P. Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title | Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title_full | Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title_fullStr | Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title_full_unstemmed | Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title_short | Mistrust and negative self‐esteem: Two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
title_sort | mistrust and negative self‐esteem: two paths from attachment styles to paranoia |
topic | Research Papers |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8451824/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33314565 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/papt.12314 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT martinezantonp mistrustandnegativeselfesteemtwopathsfromattachmentstylestoparanoia AT agostinimaximilian mistrustandnegativeselfesteemtwopathsfromattachmentstylestoparanoia AT alsuhibaniazzam mistrustandnegativeselfesteemtwopathsfromattachmentstylestoparanoia AT bentallrichardp mistrustandnegativeselfesteemtwopathsfromattachmentstylestoparanoia |