Cargando…
Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward
Higher-order conditioning involves learning causal links between multiple events, which then allows one to make novel inferences. For example, observing a correlation between two events (e.g., a neighbor wearing a particular sports jersey), later helps one make new predictions based on this knowledg...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8520911/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34671247 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.745388 |
_version_ | 1784584782599946240 |
---|---|
author | Seitz, Benjamin M. Blaisdell, Aaron P. Sharpe, Melissa J. |
author_facet | Seitz, Benjamin M. Blaisdell, Aaron P. Sharpe, Melissa J. |
author_sort | Seitz, Benjamin M. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Higher-order conditioning involves learning causal links between multiple events, which then allows one to make novel inferences. For example, observing a correlation between two events (e.g., a neighbor wearing a particular sports jersey), later helps one make new predictions based on this knowledge (e.g., the neighbor’s wife’s favorite sports team). This type of learning is important because it allows one to benefit maximally from previous experiences and perform adaptively in complex environments where many things are ambiguous or uncertain. Two procedures in the lab are often used to probe this kind of learning, second-order conditioning (SOC) and sensory preconditioning (SPC). In second-order conditioning (SOC), we first teach subjects that there is a relationship between a stimulus and an outcome (e.g., a tone that predicts food). Then, an additional stimulus is taught to precede the predictive stimulus (e.g., a light leads to the food-predictive tone). In sensory preconditioning (SPC), this order of training is reversed. Specifically, the two neutral stimuli (i.e., light and tone) are first paired together and then the tone is paired separately with food. Interestingly, in both SPC and SOC, humans, rodents, and even insects, and other invertebrates will later predict that both the light and tone are likely to lead to food, even though they only experienced the tone directly paired with food. While these processes are procedurally similar, a wealth of research suggests they are associatively and neurobiologically distinct. However, midbrain dopamine, a neurotransmitter long thought to facilitate basic Pavlovian conditioning in a relatively simplistic manner, appears critical for both SOC and SPC. These findings suggest dopamine may contribute to learning in ways that transcend differences in associative and neurological structure. We discuss how research demonstrating that dopamine is critical to both SOC and SPC places it at the center of more complex forms of cognition (e.g., spatial navigation and causal reasoning). Further, we suggest that these more sophisticated learning procedures, coupled with recent advances in recording and manipulating dopamine neurons, represent a new path forward in understanding dopamine’s contribution to learning and cognition. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8520911 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-85209112021-10-19 Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward Seitz, Benjamin M. Blaisdell, Aaron P. Sharpe, Melissa J. Front Behav Neurosci Behavioral Neuroscience Higher-order conditioning involves learning causal links between multiple events, which then allows one to make novel inferences. For example, observing a correlation between two events (e.g., a neighbor wearing a particular sports jersey), later helps one make new predictions based on this knowledge (e.g., the neighbor’s wife’s favorite sports team). This type of learning is important because it allows one to benefit maximally from previous experiences and perform adaptively in complex environments where many things are ambiguous or uncertain. Two procedures in the lab are often used to probe this kind of learning, second-order conditioning (SOC) and sensory preconditioning (SPC). In second-order conditioning (SOC), we first teach subjects that there is a relationship between a stimulus and an outcome (e.g., a tone that predicts food). Then, an additional stimulus is taught to precede the predictive stimulus (e.g., a light leads to the food-predictive tone). In sensory preconditioning (SPC), this order of training is reversed. Specifically, the two neutral stimuli (i.e., light and tone) are first paired together and then the tone is paired separately with food. Interestingly, in both SPC and SOC, humans, rodents, and even insects, and other invertebrates will later predict that both the light and tone are likely to lead to food, even though they only experienced the tone directly paired with food. While these processes are procedurally similar, a wealth of research suggests they are associatively and neurobiologically distinct. However, midbrain dopamine, a neurotransmitter long thought to facilitate basic Pavlovian conditioning in a relatively simplistic manner, appears critical for both SOC and SPC. These findings suggest dopamine may contribute to learning in ways that transcend differences in associative and neurological structure. We discuss how research demonstrating that dopamine is critical to both SOC and SPC places it at the center of more complex forms of cognition (e.g., spatial navigation and causal reasoning). Further, we suggest that these more sophisticated learning procedures, coupled with recent advances in recording and manipulating dopamine neurons, represent a new path forward in understanding dopamine’s contribution to learning and cognition. Frontiers Media S.A. 2021-10-04 /pmc/articles/PMC8520911/ /pubmed/34671247 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.745388 Text en Copyright © 2021 Seitz, Blaisdell and Sharpe. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Behavioral Neuroscience Seitz, Benjamin M. Blaisdell, Aaron P. Sharpe, Melissa J. Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title | Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title_full | Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title_fullStr | Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title_full_unstemmed | Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title_short | Higher-Order Conditioning and Dopamine: Charting a Path Forward |
title_sort | higher-order conditioning and dopamine: charting a path forward |
topic | Behavioral Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8520911/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34671247 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.745388 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT seitzbenjaminm higherorderconditioninganddopaminechartingapathforward AT blaisdellaaronp higherorderconditioninganddopaminechartingapathforward AT sharpemelissaj higherorderconditioninganddopaminechartingapathforward |