Cargando…
Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pursuits. If echoes are processed on a click-by-click basis, as assumed,...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8547948/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34696826 http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.68825 |
_version_ | 1784590479577317376 |
---|---|
author | Vance, Heather Madsen, Peter T Aguilar de Soto, Natacha Wisniewska, Danuta Maria Ladegaard, Michael Hooker, Sascha Johnson, Mark |
author_facet | Vance, Heather Madsen, Peter T Aguilar de Soto, Natacha Wisniewska, Danuta Maria Ladegaard, Michael Hooker, Sascha Johnson, Mark |
author_sort | Vance, Heather |
collection | PubMed |
description | Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pursuits. If echoes are processed on a click-by-click basis, as assumed, neural responses 100× faster than those in vision are required to keep pace with this information flow. Using high-resolution biologging of wild predator-prey interactions, we show that toothed whales adjust clicking rates to track prey movement within 50–200 ms of prey escape responses. Hypothesising that these stereotyped biosonar adjustments are elicited by sudden prey accelerations, we measured echo-kinetic responses from trained harbour porpoises to a moving target and found similar latencies. High biosonar sampling rates are, therefore, not supported by extreme speeds of neural processing and muscular responses. Instead, the neurokinetic response times in echolocation are similar to those of tracking responses in vision, suggesting a common neural underpinning. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8547948 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-85479482021-10-27 Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey Vance, Heather Madsen, Peter T Aguilar de Soto, Natacha Wisniewska, Danuta Maria Ladegaard, Michael Hooker, Sascha Johnson, Mark eLife Ecology Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pursuits. If echoes are processed on a click-by-click basis, as assumed, neural responses 100× faster than those in vision are required to keep pace with this information flow. Using high-resolution biologging of wild predator-prey interactions, we show that toothed whales adjust clicking rates to track prey movement within 50–200 ms of prey escape responses. Hypothesising that these stereotyped biosonar adjustments are elicited by sudden prey accelerations, we measured echo-kinetic responses from trained harbour porpoises to a moving target and found similar latencies. High biosonar sampling rates are, therefore, not supported by extreme speeds of neural processing and muscular responses. Instead, the neurokinetic response times in echolocation are similar to those of tracking responses in vision, suggesting a common neural underpinning. eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd 2021-10-26 /pmc/articles/PMC8547948/ /pubmed/34696826 http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.68825 Text en © 2021, Vance et al https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Ecology Vance, Heather Madsen, Peter T Aguilar de Soto, Natacha Wisniewska, Danuta Maria Ladegaard, Michael Hooker, Sascha Johnson, Mark Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title | Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title_full | Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title_fullStr | Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title_full_unstemmed | Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title_short | Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
title_sort | echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey |
topic | Ecology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8547948/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34696826 http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.68825 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT vanceheather echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT madsenpetert echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT aguilardesotonatacha echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT wisniewskadanutamaria echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT ladegaardmichael echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT hookersascha echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey AT johnsonmark echolocatingtoothedwhalesuseultrafastechokineticresponsestotrackevasiveprey |