Cargando…
Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior
This article reviews recent findings from the author’s laboratory that may provide new insights into how habits are made and broken. Habits are extensively practiced behaviors that are automatically evoked by antecedent cues and performed without their goal (or reinforcer) “in mind.” Goal-directed a...
Autor principal: | |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer US
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8602149/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34713424 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00488-z |
_version_ | 1784601516567429120 |
---|---|
author | Bouton, Mark E. |
author_facet | Bouton, Mark E. |
author_sort | Bouton, Mark E. |
collection | PubMed |
description | This article reviews recent findings from the author’s laboratory that may provide new insights into how habits are made and broken. Habits are extensively practiced behaviors that are automatically evoked by antecedent cues and performed without their goal (or reinforcer) “in mind.” Goal-directed actions, in contrast, are instrumental behaviors that are performed because their goal is remembered and valued. New results suggest that actions may transition to habit after extended practice when conditions encourage reduced attention to the behavior. Consistent with theories of attention and learning, a behavior may command less attention (and become habitual) as its reinforcer becomes well-predicted by cues in the environment; habit learning is prevented if presentation of the reinforcer is uncertain. Other results suggest that habits are not permanent, and that goal-direction can be restored by several environmental manipulations, including exposure to unexpected reinforcers or context change. Habits are more context-dependent than goal-directed actions are. Habit learning causes retroactive interference in a way that is reminiscent of extinction: It inhibits, but does not erase, goal-direction in a context-dependent way. The findings have implications for the understanding of habitual and goal-directed control of behavior as well as disordered behaviors like addictions. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8602149 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Springer US |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-86021492021-12-03 Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior Bouton, Mark E. Learn Behav Invited Review This article reviews recent findings from the author’s laboratory that may provide new insights into how habits are made and broken. Habits are extensively practiced behaviors that are automatically evoked by antecedent cues and performed without their goal (or reinforcer) “in mind.” Goal-directed actions, in contrast, are instrumental behaviors that are performed because their goal is remembered and valued. New results suggest that actions may transition to habit after extended practice when conditions encourage reduced attention to the behavior. Consistent with theories of attention and learning, a behavior may command less attention (and become habitual) as its reinforcer becomes well-predicted by cues in the environment; habit learning is prevented if presentation of the reinforcer is uncertain. Other results suggest that habits are not permanent, and that goal-direction can be restored by several environmental manipulations, including exposure to unexpected reinforcers or context change. Habits are more context-dependent than goal-directed actions are. Habit learning causes retroactive interference in a way that is reminiscent of extinction: It inhibits, but does not erase, goal-direction in a context-dependent way. The findings have implications for the understanding of habitual and goal-directed control of behavior as well as disordered behaviors like addictions. Springer US 2021-10-28 2021 /pmc/articles/PMC8602149/ /pubmed/34713424 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00488-z Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Invited Review Bouton, Mark E. Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title | Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title_full | Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title_fullStr | Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title_full_unstemmed | Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title_short | Context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
title_sort | context, attention, and the switch between habit and goal-direction in behavior |
topic | Invited Review |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8602149/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34713424 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00488-z |
work_keys_str_mv | AT boutonmarke contextattentionandtheswitchbetweenhabitandgoaldirectioninbehavior |