Cargando…

Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results

Research findings are best understood by considering contextual factors such as treatment plausibility: how likely it is that a studied treatment or manipulation is effective, based on theory and data. If a treatment is implausible, then more evidence should be required before believing it has an ef...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Thompson, W. Burt, Radell, Milen L.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: PeerJ Inc. 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8621712/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34900438
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.12532
_version_ 1784605521432543232
author Thompson, W. Burt
Radell, Milen L.
author_facet Thompson, W. Burt
Radell, Milen L.
author_sort Thompson, W. Burt
collection PubMed
description Research findings are best understood by considering contextual factors such as treatment plausibility: how likely it is that a studied treatment or manipulation is effective, based on theory and data. If a treatment is implausible, then more evidence should be required before believing it has an effect. The current study assessed the extent to which the interpretation of a research finding is affected by treatment plausibility. Participant age varied from 18 to 82 (M = 27.4, SD = 9.4), and about half of the participants (53%) were college students. A total of 600 participants read a brief news article about an experiment with a new type of psychotherapy for weight loss. The current study used a 2 (treatment plausibility) × 3 (results type) between-subjects factorial design. Treatment plausibility had two levels: (1) a plausible cognitive behavioral therapy and (2) an implausible ”psychic reinforcement therapy” that was described as employing psychic messages to promote weight loss. The three levels of the results type factor varied how the study results were presented in the article: (1) standard results with no mention of treatment plausibility, (2) standard results followed by interpretive statements focused on treatment plausibility, and (3) no results—the study was described as still in progress. Participants rated their belief in the effectiveness of the therapy on a scale of 0 to 100% in 10% increments. When treatment plausibility was not discussed in the article, average ratings for the implausible therapy were relatively high (M = 63.1%, SD = 25.0, 95% CI% [58.2–68.1]) and similar to those for the plausible therapy (M = 69.2%, SD = 21.5, 95% CI% [65.0–73.5]). Ratings for the implausible treatment were moderately lower when the article explained why the results supporting it were questionable (M = 48.5%, SD = 26.6, 95% CI% [43.2–53.8]). The findings of the current study suggest that students and other members of the public may draw incorrect inferences from research partly because they do not appreciate the importance of treatment plausibility. This could be remedied, though not completely, by explicitly discussing the plausibility of the treatment based on theory and prior data.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8621712
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher PeerJ Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-86217122021-12-09 Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results Thompson, W. Burt Radell, Milen L. PeerJ Psychiatry and Psychology Research findings are best understood by considering contextual factors such as treatment plausibility: how likely it is that a studied treatment or manipulation is effective, based on theory and data. If a treatment is implausible, then more evidence should be required before believing it has an effect. The current study assessed the extent to which the interpretation of a research finding is affected by treatment plausibility. Participant age varied from 18 to 82 (M = 27.4, SD = 9.4), and about half of the participants (53%) were college students. A total of 600 participants read a brief news article about an experiment with a new type of psychotherapy for weight loss. The current study used a 2 (treatment plausibility) × 3 (results type) between-subjects factorial design. Treatment plausibility had two levels: (1) a plausible cognitive behavioral therapy and (2) an implausible ”psychic reinforcement therapy” that was described as employing psychic messages to promote weight loss. The three levels of the results type factor varied how the study results were presented in the article: (1) standard results with no mention of treatment plausibility, (2) standard results followed by interpretive statements focused on treatment plausibility, and (3) no results—the study was described as still in progress. Participants rated their belief in the effectiveness of the therapy on a scale of 0 to 100% in 10% increments. When treatment plausibility was not discussed in the article, average ratings for the implausible therapy were relatively high (M = 63.1%, SD = 25.0, 95% CI% [58.2–68.1]) and similar to those for the plausible therapy (M = 69.2%, SD = 21.5, 95% CI% [65.0–73.5]). Ratings for the implausible treatment were moderately lower when the article explained why the results supporting it were questionable (M = 48.5%, SD = 26.6, 95% CI% [43.2–53.8]). The findings of the current study suggest that students and other members of the public may draw incorrect inferences from research partly because they do not appreciate the importance of treatment plausibility. This could be remedied, though not completely, by explicitly discussing the plausibility of the treatment based on theory and prior data. PeerJ Inc. 2021-11-23 /pmc/articles/PMC8621712/ /pubmed/34900438 http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.12532 Text en ©2021 Thompson and Radell https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited.
spellingShingle Psychiatry and Psychology
Thompson, W. Burt
Radell, Milen L.
Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title_full Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title_fullStr Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title_full_unstemmed Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title_short Acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
title_sort acceptance of anomalous research findings: explaining treatment implausibility reduces belief in far-fetched results
topic Psychiatry and Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8621712/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34900438
http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.12532
work_keys_str_mv AT thompsonwburt acceptanceofanomalousresearchfindingsexplainingtreatmentimplausibilityreducesbeliefinfarfetchedresults
AT radellmilenl acceptanceofanomalousresearchfindingsexplainingtreatmentimplausibilityreducesbeliefinfarfetchedresults