Cargando…

Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target

INTRODUCTION: Previous findings have demonstrated that several Gestalt principles do facilitate VSTM performance in change detection tasks. However, few studies have investigated the role of and time‐course of global–local consistency in motion perception. METHODS: Participants were required to trac...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Chen, Tingting, Ding, Jinhong, Yue, Guang H., Liu, Haoqiang, Li, Jie, Jiang, Changhao
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8785627/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34859605
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2444
_version_ 1784639002808156160
author Chen, Tingting
Ding, Jinhong
Yue, Guang H.
Liu, Haoqiang
Li, Jie
Jiang, Changhao
author_facet Chen, Tingting
Ding, Jinhong
Yue, Guang H.
Liu, Haoqiang
Li, Jie
Jiang, Changhao
author_sort Chen, Tingting
collection PubMed
description INTRODUCTION: Previous findings have demonstrated that several Gestalt principles do facilitate VSTM performance in change detection tasks. However, few studies have investigated the role of and time‐course of global–local consistency in motion perception. METHODS: Participants were required to track a moving target surrounded by three different backgrounds: blank, inconsistent, or consistent. Global–local objects were be bound to move together (covariation). During the PMT, participants had to follow the moving target with their eyes and react as fast as possible when the target had just vanished behind the obstruction or would arrive at a predetermined point of interception. Variable error (VE) and constant error (CE) of estimated time‐to‐contact (TTC) and gain of smooth pursuit eye movements were calculated in various conditions and analyzed qualitatively. RESULTS: Experiment 1 established the basic finding that VSTM performance could benefit from global–local consistency. Experiment 2 extended this finding by eye‐tracking device. Both in visible phase and in occluded phase, CEs were smaller for the target in a consistent background than for the target in an inconsistent background and for the target in a blank background, with both differences significant (ps < .05). However, the difference in VE among three conditions was not significant. At early stage (100–250 ms), later stage (2750–3000 ms), and termination stage (5750–6000 ms) of smooth pursuit, the velocity gains were higher in the trials with consistent backgrounds than in the trials with inconsistent backgrounds and blank backgrounds (ps < .001). With the exception of 100–250 ms phase, the means did not differ between the inconsistent background and the blank background trials (ps > .1). CONCLUSIONS: Global–local consistency could be activated within the first few hundred milliseconds to prioritize the deployment of attention and eye movement to component target. Meanwhile, it also removes ambiguity from motion tracking and TTC estimation under some unpredictable conditions, leading to the consistency advantage during smooth‐pursuit termination phase. Global–local consistency may act as an important information source to TTC estimation and oculomotor response in PMT.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8785627
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher John Wiley and Sons Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-87856272022-01-31 Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target Chen, Tingting Ding, Jinhong Yue, Guang H. Liu, Haoqiang Li, Jie Jiang, Changhao Brain Behav Original Articles INTRODUCTION: Previous findings have demonstrated that several Gestalt principles do facilitate VSTM performance in change detection tasks. However, few studies have investigated the role of and time‐course of global–local consistency in motion perception. METHODS: Participants were required to track a moving target surrounded by three different backgrounds: blank, inconsistent, or consistent. Global–local objects were be bound to move together (covariation). During the PMT, participants had to follow the moving target with their eyes and react as fast as possible when the target had just vanished behind the obstruction or would arrive at a predetermined point of interception. Variable error (VE) and constant error (CE) of estimated time‐to‐contact (TTC) and gain of smooth pursuit eye movements were calculated in various conditions and analyzed qualitatively. RESULTS: Experiment 1 established the basic finding that VSTM performance could benefit from global–local consistency. Experiment 2 extended this finding by eye‐tracking device. Both in visible phase and in occluded phase, CEs were smaller for the target in a consistent background than for the target in an inconsistent background and for the target in a blank background, with both differences significant (ps < .05). However, the difference in VE among three conditions was not significant. At early stage (100–250 ms), later stage (2750–3000 ms), and termination stage (5750–6000 ms) of smooth pursuit, the velocity gains were higher in the trials with consistent backgrounds than in the trials with inconsistent backgrounds and blank backgrounds (ps < .001). With the exception of 100–250 ms phase, the means did not differ between the inconsistent background and the blank background trials (ps > .1). CONCLUSIONS: Global–local consistency could be activated within the first few hundred milliseconds to prioritize the deployment of attention and eye movement to component target. Meanwhile, it also removes ambiguity from motion tracking and TTC estimation under some unpredictable conditions, leading to the consistency advantage during smooth‐pursuit termination phase. Global–local consistency may act as an important information source to TTC estimation and oculomotor response in PMT. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2021-12-03 /pmc/articles/PMC8785627/ /pubmed/34859605 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2444 Text en © 2021 The Authors. Brain and Behavior published by Wiley Periodicals LLC https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Original Articles
Chen, Tingting
Ding, Jinhong
Yue, Guang H.
Liu, Haoqiang
Li, Jie
Jiang, Changhao
Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title_full Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title_fullStr Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title_full_unstemmed Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title_short Global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
title_sort global–local consistency benefits memory‐guided tracking of a moving target
topic Original Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8785627/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34859605
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2444
work_keys_str_mv AT chentingting globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget
AT dingjinhong globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget
AT yueguangh globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget
AT liuhaoqiang globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget
AT lijie globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget
AT jiangchanghao globallocalconsistencybenefitsmemoryguidedtrackingofamovingtarget