Cargando…
Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs
Facial expressions potentially serve as indicators of animal emotions if they are consistently present across situations that (likely) elicit the same emotional state. In a previous study, we used the Dog Facial Action Coding System (DogFACS) to identify facial expressions in dogs associated with co...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8904359/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34338869 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1 |
_version_ | 1784664930017869824 |
---|---|
author | Bremhorst, A. Mills, D. S. Würbel, H. Riemer, S. |
author_facet | Bremhorst, A. Mills, D. S. Würbel, H. Riemer, S. |
author_sort | Bremhorst, A. |
collection | PubMed |
description | Facial expressions potentially serve as indicators of animal emotions if they are consistently present across situations that (likely) elicit the same emotional state. In a previous study, we used the Dog Facial Action Coding System (DogFACS) to identify facial expressions in dogs associated with conditions presumably eliciting positive anticipation (expectation of a food reward) and frustration (prevention of access to the food). Our first aim here was to identify facial expressions of positive anticipation and frustration in dogs that are context-independent (and thus have potential as emotion indicators) and to distinguish them from expressions that are reward-specific (and thus might relate to a motivational state associated with the expected reward). Therefore, we tested a new sample of 28 dogs with a similar set-up designed to induce positive anticipation (positive condition) and frustration (negative condition) in two reward contexts: food and toys. The previous results were replicated: Ears adductor was associated with the positive condition and Ears flattener, Blink, Lips part, Jaw drop, and Nose lick with the negative condition. Four additional facial actions were also more common in the negative condition. All actions except the Upper lip raiser were independent of reward type. Our second aim was to assess basic measures of diagnostic accuracy for the potential emotion indicators. Ears flattener and Ears downward had relatively high sensitivity but low specificity, whereas the opposite was the case for the other negative correlates. Ears adductor had excellent specificity but low sensitivity. If the identified facial expressions were to be used individually as diagnostic indicators, none would allow consistent correct classifications of the associated emotion. Diagnostic accuracy measures are an essential feature for validity assessments of potential indicators of animal emotion. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-8904359 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-89043592022-03-15 Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs Bremhorst, A. Mills, D. S. Würbel, H. Riemer, S. Anim Cogn Original Paper Facial expressions potentially serve as indicators of animal emotions if they are consistently present across situations that (likely) elicit the same emotional state. In a previous study, we used the Dog Facial Action Coding System (DogFACS) to identify facial expressions in dogs associated with conditions presumably eliciting positive anticipation (expectation of a food reward) and frustration (prevention of access to the food). Our first aim here was to identify facial expressions of positive anticipation and frustration in dogs that are context-independent (and thus have potential as emotion indicators) and to distinguish them from expressions that are reward-specific (and thus might relate to a motivational state associated with the expected reward). Therefore, we tested a new sample of 28 dogs with a similar set-up designed to induce positive anticipation (positive condition) and frustration (negative condition) in two reward contexts: food and toys. The previous results were replicated: Ears adductor was associated with the positive condition and Ears flattener, Blink, Lips part, Jaw drop, and Nose lick with the negative condition. Four additional facial actions were also more common in the negative condition. All actions except the Upper lip raiser were independent of reward type. Our second aim was to assess basic measures of diagnostic accuracy for the potential emotion indicators. Ears flattener and Ears downward had relatively high sensitivity but low specificity, whereas the opposite was the case for the other negative correlates. Ears adductor had excellent specificity but low sensitivity. If the identified facial expressions were to be used individually as diagnostic indicators, none would allow consistent correct classifications of the associated emotion. Diagnostic accuracy measures are an essential feature for validity assessments of potential indicators of animal emotion. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1. Springer Berlin Heidelberg 2021-08-02 2022 /pmc/articles/PMC8904359/ /pubmed/34338869 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Original Paper Bremhorst, A. Mills, D. S. Würbel, H. Riemer, S. Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title | Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title_full | Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title_fullStr | Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title_full_unstemmed | Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title_short | Evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
title_sort | evaluating the accuracy of facial expressions as emotion indicators across contexts in dogs |
topic | Original Paper |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8904359/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34338869 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01532-1 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT bremhorsta evaluatingtheaccuracyoffacialexpressionsasemotionindicatorsacrosscontextsindogs AT millsds evaluatingtheaccuracyoffacialexpressionsasemotionindicatorsacrosscontextsindogs AT wurbelh evaluatingtheaccuracyoffacialexpressionsasemotionindicatorsacrosscontextsindogs AT riemers evaluatingtheaccuracyoffacialexpressionsasemotionindicatorsacrosscontextsindogs |