Cargando…

Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location

Traplining, when animals repeat the order in which they visit a number of locations, is taxonomically widespread, but little is known about which factors influence the routes that animals follow. For example, as the quality of rewarding locations changes over time, foragers are expected to update th...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Tello-Ramos, Maria C., Hurly, T. Andrew, Barclay, Mabel, Healy, Susan D.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer US 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8979907/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34341946
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00476-3
_version_ 1784681280077561856
author Tello-Ramos, Maria C.
Hurly, T. Andrew
Barclay, Mabel
Healy, Susan D.
author_facet Tello-Ramos, Maria C.
Hurly, T. Andrew
Barclay, Mabel
Healy, Susan D.
author_sort Tello-Ramos, Maria C.
collection PubMed
description Traplining, when animals repeat the order in which they visit a number of locations, is taxonomically widespread, but little is known about which factors influence the routes that animals follow. For example, as the quality of rewarding locations changes over time, foragers are expected to update their traplines, either to prioritize locations where the reward increases or to avoid locations that have ceased to be profitable. Here, we tested how traplining wild hummingbirds responded to increases or to decreases in the sucrose concentration of one of the flowers on their trapline. Hummingbirds did not change their trapline to visit the flower with the increased reward first, but by changing the order in which they visited flowers, they avoided a flower that contained a decreased reward. Depending on where along the trapline the reduced-content flower occurred, hummingbirds either changed the origin of their trapline or changed the direction in which they flew around their trapline. It may be that this asymmetric modification of foraging traplines is especially noticeable in risk-averse foragers, such as these territorial hummingbirds. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.3758/s13420-021-00476-3.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8979907
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher Springer US
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-89799072022-04-22 Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location Tello-Ramos, Maria C. Hurly, T. Andrew Barclay, Mabel Healy, Susan D. Learn Behav Article Traplining, when animals repeat the order in which they visit a number of locations, is taxonomically widespread, but little is known about which factors influence the routes that animals follow. For example, as the quality of rewarding locations changes over time, foragers are expected to update their traplines, either to prioritize locations where the reward increases or to avoid locations that have ceased to be profitable. Here, we tested how traplining wild hummingbirds responded to increases or to decreases in the sucrose concentration of one of the flowers on their trapline. Hummingbirds did not change their trapline to visit the flower with the increased reward first, but by changing the order in which they visited flowers, they avoided a flower that contained a decreased reward. Depending on where along the trapline the reduced-content flower occurred, hummingbirds either changed the origin of their trapline or changed the direction in which they flew around their trapline. It may be that this asymmetric modification of foraging traplines is especially noticeable in risk-averse foragers, such as these territorial hummingbirds. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.3758/s13420-021-00476-3. Springer US 2021-08-02 2022 /pmc/articles/PMC8979907/ /pubmed/34341946 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00476-3 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Article
Tello-Ramos, Maria C.
Hurly, T. Andrew
Barclay, Mabel
Healy, Susan D.
Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title_full Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title_fullStr Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title_full_unstemmed Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title_short Hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
title_sort hummingbirds modify their routes to avoid a poor location
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8979907/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34341946
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00476-3
work_keys_str_mv AT telloramosmariac hummingbirdsmodifytheirroutestoavoidapoorlocation
AT hurlytandrew hummingbirdsmodifytheirroutestoavoidapoorlocation
AT barclaymabel hummingbirdsmodifytheirroutestoavoidapoorlocation
AT healysusand hummingbirdsmodifytheirroutestoavoidapoorlocation