Cargando…

Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching

Repetition of specific movement biases subsequent actions towards the practiced movement, a phenomenon known as use-dependent learning (UDL). Recent experiments that impose strict constraints on planning time have revealed two sources of use-dependent biases, one arising from dynamic changes occurri...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Tsay, Jonathan S., Kim, Hyosub E., Saxena, Arohi, Parvin, Darius E., Verstynen, Timothy, Ivry, Richard B.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: The Royal Society 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9043705/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35473382
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0415
_version_ 1784694940683468800
author Tsay, Jonathan S.
Kim, Hyosub E.
Saxena, Arohi
Parvin, Darius E.
Verstynen, Timothy
Ivry, Richard B.
author_facet Tsay, Jonathan S.
Kim, Hyosub E.
Saxena, Arohi
Parvin, Darius E.
Verstynen, Timothy
Ivry, Richard B.
author_sort Tsay, Jonathan S.
collection PubMed
description Repetition of specific movement biases subsequent actions towards the practiced movement, a phenomenon known as use-dependent learning (UDL). Recent experiments that impose strict constraints on planning time have revealed two sources of use-dependent biases, one arising from dynamic changes occurring during motor planning and another reflecting a stable shift in motor execution. Here, we used a distributional analysis to examine the contribution of these biases in reaching. To create the conditions for UDL, the target appeared at a designated ‘frequent’ location on most trials, and at one of six ‘rare’ locations on other trials. Strikingly, the heading angles were bimodally distributed, with peaks at both frequent and rare target locations. Despite having no constraints on planning time, participants exhibited a robust bias towards the frequent target when movements were self-initiated quickly, the signature of a planning bias; notably, the peak near the rare target was shifted in the frequently practiced direction, the signature of an execution bias. Furthermore, these execution biases were not only replicated in a delayed-response task but were also insensitive to reward. Taken together, these results extend our understanding of how volitional movements are influenced by recent experience.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9043705
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher The Royal Society
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-90437052022-05-10 Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching Tsay, Jonathan S. Kim, Hyosub E. Saxena, Arohi Parvin, Darius E. Verstynen, Timothy Ivry, Richard B. Proc Biol Sci Behaviour Repetition of specific movement biases subsequent actions towards the practiced movement, a phenomenon known as use-dependent learning (UDL). Recent experiments that impose strict constraints on planning time have revealed two sources of use-dependent biases, one arising from dynamic changes occurring during motor planning and another reflecting a stable shift in motor execution. Here, we used a distributional analysis to examine the contribution of these biases in reaching. To create the conditions for UDL, the target appeared at a designated ‘frequent’ location on most trials, and at one of six ‘rare’ locations on other trials. Strikingly, the heading angles were bimodally distributed, with peaks at both frequent and rare target locations. Despite having no constraints on planning time, participants exhibited a robust bias towards the frequent target when movements were self-initiated quickly, the signature of a planning bias; notably, the peak near the rare target was shifted in the frequently practiced direction, the signature of an execution bias. Furthermore, these execution biases were not only replicated in a delayed-response task but were also insensitive to reward. Taken together, these results extend our understanding of how volitional movements are influenced by recent experience. The Royal Society 2022-04-27 2022-04-27 /pmc/articles/PMC9043705/ /pubmed/35473382 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0415 Text en © 2022 The Authors. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Behaviour
Tsay, Jonathan S.
Kim, Hyosub E.
Saxena, Arohi
Parvin, Darius E.
Verstynen, Timothy
Ivry, Richard B.
Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title_full Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title_fullStr Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title_full_unstemmed Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title_short Dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
title_sort dissociable use-dependent processes for volitional goal-directed reaching
topic Behaviour
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9043705/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35473382
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0415
work_keys_str_mv AT tsayjonathans dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching
AT kimhyosube dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching
AT saxenaarohi dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching
AT parvindariuse dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching
AT verstynentimothy dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching
AT ivryrichardb dissociableusedependentprocessesforvolitionalgoaldirectedreaching