Cargando…
Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study
Odor context can affect the recognition of facial expressions. However, there is no evidence to date that odor can regulate the processing of emotional words conveyed by visual words. An emotional word recognition task was combined with event-related potential technology. Briefly, 49 adults were ran...
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer Netherlands
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9120329/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35603047 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11571-021-09733-7 |
_version_ | 1784710902051766272 |
---|---|
author | Li, Danyang Wang, Xiaochun |
author_facet | Li, Danyang Wang, Xiaochun |
author_sort | Li, Danyang |
collection | PubMed |
description | Odor context can affect the recognition of facial expressions. However, there is no evidence to date that odor can regulate the processing of emotional words conveyed by visual words. An emotional word recognition task was combined with event-related potential technology. Briefly, 49 adults were randomly divided into three odor contexts (pleasant odor, unpleasant odor, and no odor) to judge the valence of emotional words (positive, negative, and neutral). Both behavioral and Electroencephalography (EEG) data were collected. Both the pleasant odor and unpleasant odor contexts shortened the response time of the subjects to emotional words. In addition, negative words induced greater amplitudes of early posterior negativity (EPN) and late positive potential (LPP) than the positive and neutral words. However, the neutral words induced a larger N400 amplitude than the positive and negative words. More importantly, the processing of emotional words was found to be modulated by external odor contexts. For example, during the earlier (P2) processing stages, pleasant and unpleasant odor contexts induced greater P2 amplitudes than the no odor context. In the unpleasant odor context, negative words with the same odor valence induced greater P2 amplitudes than the positive words. During the later (N400) stages, various regions of the brain regions exhibited different results. For example, in the left and right frontal areas of the brain, exposure to positive words in a pleasant odor context resulted in a smaller N400 amplitude than exposure to neutral words in the same context. Meanwhile, in the left and right central regions, emotional words with the same valence as pleasant or unpleasant odor contexts elicited the minimum N400 amplitude. Individuals are very sensitive to emotional information. With deeper processing, different cognitive processes are reflected and they can be modulated by external odors. In the early and late stages of word processing, both pleasant and unpleasant odor contexts exhibited an undifferentiated dominance effect and could specifically modulate affectively congruent words. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9120329 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Springer Netherlands |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-91203292022-05-21 Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study Li, Danyang Wang, Xiaochun Cogn Neurodyn Research Article Odor context can affect the recognition of facial expressions. However, there is no evidence to date that odor can regulate the processing of emotional words conveyed by visual words. An emotional word recognition task was combined with event-related potential technology. Briefly, 49 adults were randomly divided into three odor contexts (pleasant odor, unpleasant odor, and no odor) to judge the valence of emotional words (positive, negative, and neutral). Both behavioral and Electroencephalography (EEG) data were collected. Both the pleasant odor and unpleasant odor contexts shortened the response time of the subjects to emotional words. In addition, negative words induced greater amplitudes of early posterior negativity (EPN) and late positive potential (LPP) than the positive and neutral words. However, the neutral words induced a larger N400 amplitude than the positive and negative words. More importantly, the processing of emotional words was found to be modulated by external odor contexts. For example, during the earlier (P2) processing stages, pleasant and unpleasant odor contexts induced greater P2 amplitudes than the no odor context. In the unpleasant odor context, negative words with the same odor valence induced greater P2 amplitudes than the positive words. During the later (N400) stages, various regions of the brain regions exhibited different results. For example, in the left and right frontal areas of the brain, exposure to positive words in a pleasant odor context resulted in a smaller N400 amplitude than exposure to neutral words in the same context. Meanwhile, in the left and right central regions, emotional words with the same valence as pleasant or unpleasant odor contexts elicited the minimum N400 amplitude. Individuals are very sensitive to emotional information. With deeper processing, different cognitive processes are reflected and they can be modulated by external odors. In the early and late stages of word processing, both pleasant and unpleasant odor contexts exhibited an undifferentiated dominance effect and could specifically modulate affectively congruent words. Springer Netherlands 2021-11-02 2022-06 /pmc/articles/PMC9120329/ /pubmed/35603047 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11571-021-09733-7 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Research Article Li, Danyang Wang, Xiaochun Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title | Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title_full | Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title_fullStr | Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title_full_unstemmed | Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title_short | Can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? A behavioral and event-related potentials study |
title_sort | can ambient odors influence the recognition of emotional words? a behavioral and event-related potentials study |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9120329/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35603047 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11571-021-09733-7 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT lidanyang canambientodorsinfluencetherecognitionofemotionalwordsabehavioralandeventrelatedpotentialsstudy AT wangxiaochun canambientodorsinfluencetherecognitionofemotionalwordsabehavioralandeventrelatedpotentialsstudy |