Cargando…
Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys
This research examines whether the mere presence of asking about gender pronouns (e.g., she/her, he/him, they/them, and ze/zir) in a survey enhances participants’ attitudes and satisfaction of answering the questions. A large sample (N = 1,511) of heterosexual, cisgender, and LGBTQIA+ participants a...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9125176/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35615163 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.873442 |
_version_ | 1784711889675091968 |
---|---|
author | Palanica, Adam Lopez, Luke Gomez, Amy Fossat, Yan |
author_facet | Palanica, Adam Lopez, Luke Gomez, Amy Fossat, Yan |
author_sort | Palanica, Adam |
collection | PubMed |
description | This research examines whether the mere presence of asking about gender pronouns (e.g., she/her, he/him, they/them, and ze/zir) in a survey enhances participants’ attitudes and satisfaction of answering the questions. A large sample (N = 1,511) of heterosexual, cisgender, and LGBTQIA+ participants across the United States (US) were surveyed an online “personality test” (as a deception), with the real purpose of examining whether asking a pronoun question enhanced their perceptions of the survey. Three demographic groups were included: (i) heterosexual–cisgender (n = 503), (ii) gay–cisgender (n = 509), and (iii) genderqueer (trans, non-conforming, other, n = 499). Half of each group were randomly given either a survey that included a gender pronoun question (test) or not (control), and then all rated their perceptions of the survey questions. For participants who identified as heterosexual or gay, no major differences were found between survey conditions. However, participants who identified as genderqueer experienced significant increases of satisfaction, comfort level, and perceived relevance of the questions when given a survey that asked their gender pronouns versus the survey that did not. These findings have implications for any surveys that ask about personal demographics, and suggest that any form of written communication should include clarity about gender pronouns. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9125176 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-91251762022-05-24 Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys Palanica, Adam Lopez, Luke Gomez, Amy Fossat, Yan Front Psychol Psychology This research examines whether the mere presence of asking about gender pronouns (e.g., she/her, he/him, they/them, and ze/zir) in a survey enhances participants’ attitudes and satisfaction of answering the questions. A large sample (N = 1,511) of heterosexual, cisgender, and LGBTQIA+ participants across the United States (US) were surveyed an online “personality test” (as a deception), with the real purpose of examining whether asking a pronoun question enhanced their perceptions of the survey. Three demographic groups were included: (i) heterosexual–cisgender (n = 503), (ii) gay–cisgender (n = 509), and (iii) genderqueer (trans, non-conforming, other, n = 499). Half of each group were randomly given either a survey that included a gender pronoun question (test) or not (control), and then all rated their perceptions of the survey questions. For participants who identified as heterosexual or gay, no major differences were found between survey conditions. However, participants who identified as genderqueer experienced significant increases of satisfaction, comfort level, and perceived relevance of the questions when given a survey that asked their gender pronouns versus the survey that did not. These findings have implications for any surveys that ask about personal demographics, and suggest that any form of written communication should include clarity about gender pronouns. Frontiers Media S.A. 2022-05-09 /pmc/articles/PMC9125176/ /pubmed/35615163 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.873442 Text en Copyright © 2022 Palanica, Lopez, Gomez and Fossat. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Psychology Palanica, Adam Lopez, Luke Gomez, Amy Fossat, Yan Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title | Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title_full | Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title_fullStr | Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title_full_unstemmed | Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title_short | Effects of Including Gender Pronoun Questions in Surveys |
title_sort | effects of including gender pronoun questions in surveys |
topic | Psychology |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9125176/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35615163 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.873442 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT palanicaadam effectsofincludinggenderpronounquestionsinsurveys AT lopezluke effectsofincludinggenderpronounquestionsinsurveys AT gomezamy effectsofincludinggenderpronounquestionsinsurveys AT fossatyan effectsofincludinggenderpronounquestionsinsurveys |