Cargando…
Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution
We typically slow down after committing an error, an effect termed post-error slowing (PES). Traditionally, PES has been calculated by subtracting post-correct from post-error RTs. Dutilh et al. (Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(3), 208-216, 2012), however, showed PES values calculated in this...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer US
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9170639/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34713426 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01673-8 |
_version_ | 1784721478611107840 |
---|---|
author | Derrfuss, Jan Danielmeier, Claudia Klein, Tilmann A. Fischer, Adrian G. Ullsperger, Markus |
author_facet | Derrfuss, Jan Danielmeier, Claudia Klein, Tilmann A. Fischer, Adrian G. Ullsperger, Markus |
author_sort | Derrfuss, Jan |
collection | PubMed |
description | We typically slow down after committing an error, an effect termed post-error slowing (PES). Traditionally, PES has been calculated by subtracting post-correct from post-error RTs. Dutilh et al. (Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(3), 208-216, 2012), however, showed PES values calculated in this way are potentially biased. Therefore, they proposed to compute robust PES scores by subtracting pre-error RTs from post-error RTs. Based on data from a large-scale study using the flanker task, we show that both traditional and robust PES estimates can be biased. The source of the bias are differential imbalances in the percentage of congruent vs. incongruent post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials. Specifically, we found that post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials were more likely to be congruent than incongruent, with the size of the imbalance depending on the trial type as well as the length of the response-stimulus interval (RSI). In our study, for trials preceded by a 700-ms RSI, the percentages of congruent trials were 62% for post-correct trials, 66% for pre-error trials, and 56% for post-error trials. Relative to unbiased estimates, these imbalances inflated traditional PES estimates by 37% (9 ms) and robust PES estimates by 42% (16 ms) when individual-participant means were calculated. When individual-participant medians were calculated, the biases were even more pronounced (40% and 50% inflation, respectively). To obtain unbiased PES scores for interference tasks, we propose to compute unweighted individual-participant means by initially calculating mean RTs for congruent and incongruent trials separately, before averaging congruent and incongruent mean RTs to calculate means for post-correct, pre-error and post-error trials. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9170639 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2021 |
publisher | Springer US |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-91706392022-06-08 Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution Derrfuss, Jan Danielmeier, Claudia Klein, Tilmann A. Fischer, Adrian G. Ullsperger, Markus Behav Res Methods Article We typically slow down after committing an error, an effect termed post-error slowing (PES). Traditionally, PES has been calculated by subtracting post-correct from post-error RTs. Dutilh et al. (Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(3), 208-216, 2012), however, showed PES values calculated in this way are potentially biased. Therefore, they proposed to compute robust PES scores by subtracting pre-error RTs from post-error RTs. Based on data from a large-scale study using the flanker task, we show that both traditional and robust PES estimates can be biased. The source of the bias are differential imbalances in the percentage of congruent vs. incongruent post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials. Specifically, we found that post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials were more likely to be congruent than incongruent, with the size of the imbalance depending on the trial type as well as the length of the response-stimulus interval (RSI). In our study, for trials preceded by a 700-ms RSI, the percentages of congruent trials were 62% for post-correct trials, 66% for pre-error trials, and 56% for post-error trials. Relative to unbiased estimates, these imbalances inflated traditional PES estimates by 37% (9 ms) and robust PES estimates by 42% (16 ms) when individual-participant means were calculated. When individual-participant medians were calculated, the biases were even more pronounced (40% and 50% inflation, respectively). To obtain unbiased PES scores for interference tasks, we propose to compute unweighted individual-participant means by initially calculating mean RTs for congruent and incongruent trials separately, before averaging congruent and incongruent mean RTs to calculate means for post-correct, pre-error and post-error trials. Springer US 2021-10-28 2022 /pmc/articles/PMC9170639/ /pubmed/34713426 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01673-8 Text en © The Author(s) 2021 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Article Derrfuss, Jan Danielmeier, Claudia Klein, Tilmann A. Fischer, Adrian G. Ullsperger, Markus Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title | Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title_full | Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title_fullStr | Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title_full_unstemmed | Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title_short | Unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: A confound and a simple solution |
title_sort | unbiased post-error slowing in interference tasks: a confound and a simple solution |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9170639/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34713426 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01673-8 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT derrfussjan unbiasedposterrorslowingininterferencetasksaconfoundandasimplesolution AT danielmeierclaudia unbiasedposterrorslowingininterferencetasksaconfoundandasimplesolution AT kleintilmanna unbiasedposterrorslowingininterferencetasksaconfoundandasimplesolution AT fischeradriang unbiasedposterrorslowingininterferencetasksaconfoundandasimplesolution AT ullspergermarkus unbiasedposterrorslowingininterferencetasksaconfoundandasimplesolution |