Cargando…

Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging

SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animals living in nests leave their nests to search for food and often use constant routes. We tested how workers of ant colonies cope with pitfall traps placed on their way to food. Such pits can represent those dug by the ant-hunting pit-building antlions. The pitfall traps delayed...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Bar, Adi, Marom, Chen, Zorin, Nikol, Gilad, Tomer, Subach, Aziz, Foitzik, Susanne, Scharf, Inon
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: MDPI 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9219994/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741418
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology11060897
_version_ 1784732261113921536
author Bar, Adi
Marom, Chen
Zorin, Nikol
Gilad, Tomer
Subach, Aziz
Foitzik, Susanne
Scharf, Inon
author_facet Bar, Adi
Marom, Chen
Zorin, Nikol
Gilad, Tomer
Subach, Aziz
Foitzik, Susanne
Scharf, Inon
author_sort Bar, Adi
collection PubMed
description SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animals living in nests leave their nests to search for food and often use constant routes. We tested how workers of ant colonies cope with pitfall traps placed on their way to food. Such pits can represent those dug by the ant-hunting pit-building antlions. The pitfall traps delayed the arrival at the food and increased the workers’ tracks, but the ants improved in searching after accumulating experience. Furthermore, workers learned to avoid falling into the pits with experience. Removing or adding pits led to a fast change in the worker behavior and they ignored the past conditions, except for tracks that were longer than expected, after pitfall traps were removed. The ants fell much more frequently into pits closer to the arena entry, suggesting that such positions are especially profitable for sit-and-wait predators, ambushing such ants. ABSTRACT: Central-place foragers, such as social insects or nesting birds, repeatedly use the same routes from and to their nests when foraging for food. Such species forage more efficiently after accumulating experience. We examined, here, a relatively neglected aspect of such an improvement with experience—the avoidance of pitfall traps. Similar pits are built by antlions, which co-occur with the ants, but they also resemble other natural obstacles. We used the desert ant Cataglyphis niger, common in sandy habitats, and allowed it to forage for three successive runs for a food reward. Ant workers discovered food more slowly and in smaller numbers when pits were in their path. Pit presence also led to longer tracks by ants and slower movement. However, with experience, the ants fell into such pits less often and reached the food more quickly. To understand how past conditions affect current behavior, we investigated whether removing or adding pits led to a different result to that with a constant number of pits. Workers adjusted their behavior immediately when conditions changed. The only carryover effect was the longer tracks crossed by workers after pit removal, possibly resulting from the mismatch between the past and current conditions. Finally, the workers were more likely to fall into pits that were closer to the nest than those that were further away. This is a good example of the advantage that ambush predators can derive from ambushing their prey in specific locations.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9219994
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher MDPI
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-92199942022-06-24 Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging Bar, Adi Marom, Chen Zorin, Nikol Gilad, Tomer Subach, Aziz Foitzik, Susanne Scharf, Inon Biology (Basel) Article SIMPLE SUMMARY: Animals living in nests leave their nests to search for food and often use constant routes. We tested how workers of ant colonies cope with pitfall traps placed on their way to food. Such pits can represent those dug by the ant-hunting pit-building antlions. The pitfall traps delayed the arrival at the food and increased the workers’ tracks, but the ants improved in searching after accumulating experience. Furthermore, workers learned to avoid falling into the pits with experience. Removing or adding pits led to a fast change in the worker behavior and they ignored the past conditions, except for tracks that were longer than expected, after pitfall traps were removed. The ants fell much more frequently into pits closer to the arena entry, suggesting that such positions are especially profitable for sit-and-wait predators, ambushing such ants. ABSTRACT: Central-place foragers, such as social insects or nesting birds, repeatedly use the same routes from and to their nests when foraging for food. Such species forage more efficiently after accumulating experience. We examined, here, a relatively neglected aspect of such an improvement with experience—the avoidance of pitfall traps. Similar pits are built by antlions, which co-occur with the ants, but they also resemble other natural obstacles. We used the desert ant Cataglyphis niger, common in sandy habitats, and allowed it to forage for three successive runs for a food reward. Ant workers discovered food more slowly and in smaller numbers when pits were in their path. Pit presence also led to longer tracks by ants and slower movement. However, with experience, the ants fell into such pits less often and reached the food more quickly. To understand how past conditions affect current behavior, we investigated whether removing or adding pits led to a different result to that with a constant number of pits. Workers adjusted their behavior immediately when conditions changed. The only carryover effect was the longer tracks crossed by workers after pit removal, possibly resulting from the mismatch between the past and current conditions. Finally, the workers were more likely to fall into pits that were closer to the nest than those that were further away. This is a good example of the advantage that ambush predators can derive from ambushing their prey in specific locations. MDPI 2022-06-10 /pmc/articles/PMC9219994/ /pubmed/35741418 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology11060897 Text en © 2022 by the authors. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
Bar, Adi
Marom, Chen
Zorin, Nikol
Gilad, Tomer
Subach, Aziz
Foitzik, Susanne
Scharf, Inon
Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title_full Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title_fullStr Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title_full_unstemmed Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title_short Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging
title_sort desert ants learn to avoid pitfall traps while foraging
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9219994/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741418
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biology11060897
work_keys_str_mv AT baradi desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT maromchen desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT zorinnikol desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT giladtomer desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT subachaziz desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT foitziksusanne desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging
AT scharfinon desertantslearntoavoidpitfalltrapswhileforaging