Cargando…

Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability

Large‐scale genomic studies have identified over 100 genes associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD); however, important phenotypic variables are captured inconsistently. In many cases, the resources required for comprehensive characterization hinder the feasibility of collecting critical infor...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Levy, Tess, Britvan, Bari, Grosman, Hannah, Giserman‐Kiss, Ivy, Meyering, Kristin, Weissman, Jordana, Halpern, Danielle, Zweifach, Jessica, Trelles, M. Pilar, Foss‐Feig, Jennifer H., Kolevzon, Alexander, Sanders, Stephan J., Robinson, Elise B., Buxbaum, Joseph D., Bishop, Somer, Siper, Paige M.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9314947/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.2704
_version_ 1784754440984592384
author Levy, Tess
Britvan, Bari
Grosman, Hannah
Giserman‐Kiss, Ivy
Meyering, Kristin
Weissman, Jordana
Halpern, Danielle
Zweifach, Jessica
Trelles, M. Pilar
Foss‐Feig, Jennifer H.
Kolevzon, Alexander
Sanders, Stephan J.
Robinson, Elise B.
Buxbaum, Joseph D.
Bishop, Somer
Siper, Paige M.
author_facet Levy, Tess
Britvan, Bari
Grosman, Hannah
Giserman‐Kiss, Ivy
Meyering, Kristin
Weissman, Jordana
Halpern, Danielle
Zweifach, Jessica
Trelles, M. Pilar
Foss‐Feig, Jennifer H.
Kolevzon, Alexander
Sanders, Stephan J.
Robinson, Elise B.
Buxbaum, Joseph D.
Bishop, Somer
Siper, Paige M.
author_sort Levy, Tess
collection PubMed
description Large‐scale genomic studies have identified over 100 genes associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD); however, important phenotypic variables are captured inconsistently. In many cases, the resources required for comprehensive characterization hinder the feasibility of collecting critical information, such as intellectual ability. Thus, electronic collection of important phenotypes would greatly facilitate large‐scale data collection efforts. This study assessed the utility of two electronic assessments as a proxy of cognitive ability relative to clinician‐administered cognitive assessments. Ninety‐two participants completed the study, including individuals with ASD (probands, n = 19), parents of probands (n = 46), and siblings without ASD (n = 27). Participants were administered the electronic‐Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (e‐PPVT‐4), an electronic visual reasoning (VR) test, and a clinician‐administered Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI‐II). Probands also completed a full, in‐person, cognitive assessment and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition. Correlations between scores on electronic and clinician‐administered measures were examined. Classification accuracy of individual scores based on 95% confidence intervals and score range (below average, average, above average) were also assessed. Moderate to strong correlations were identified between both electronic measures and the clinician‐administered WASI‐II (ρ = 0.606–0.712). Mean difference between standard scores ranged from 10.7 to 14.8 for the cohort. Classification accuracy based on WASI‐II 95% confidence interval was consistently low (27.5%–47.3%). Classification accuracy by score range (below average, average, above average) was variable, ranging from 33% to 86% for probands. All participants unable to complete the electronic assessments met DSM‐5 criteria for intellectual disability. e‐PPVT‐4 and VR scores were strongly correlated with scores on the WASI‐II full‐scale IQ (ρ = 0.630, 0.712), indicating utility of these measures at the group level in large‐scale genomic studies. However, the poor precision of measurement across both measures suggests that the e‐PPVT‐4 and VR are not useful alternatives to in‐person testing for the purpose of clinical assessment of an individual's IQ score. LAY SUMMARY: Large‐scale studies designed to identify genes associated with autism have been successful in identifying over 100 genes. However, important clinical information about participants with autism and their family members is often missed—including cognitive functioning. Cognitive testing requires in‐person administration by a trained clinician and therefore can be burdensome and often reduces feasibility of diverse samples. Here, we assessed whether electronic assessments could take the place of in‐person cognitive testing. We found that at the group level, for large‐scale studies, electronic measures added valuable information; however, they were not accurate enough to be used on an individual level (i.e., to offer feedback about an individual's predicted IQ score).
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9314947
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-93149472022-07-30 Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability Levy, Tess Britvan, Bari Grosman, Hannah Giserman‐Kiss, Ivy Meyering, Kristin Weissman, Jordana Halpern, Danielle Zweifach, Jessica Trelles, M. Pilar Foss‐Feig, Jennifer H. Kolevzon, Alexander Sanders, Stephan J. Robinson, Elise B. Buxbaum, Joseph D. Bishop, Somer Siper, Paige M. Autism Res OMICS Large‐scale genomic studies have identified over 100 genes associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD); however, important phenotypic variables are captured inconsistently. In many cases, the resources required for comprehensive characterization hinder the feasibility of collecting critical information, such as intellectual ability. Thus, electronic collection of important phenotypes would greatly facilitate large‐scale data collection efforts. This study assessed the utility of two electronic assessments as a proxy of cognitive ability relative to clinician‐administered cognitive assessments. Ninety‐two participants completed the study, including individuals with ASD (probands, n = 19), parents of probands (n = 46), and siblings without ASD (n = 27). Participants were administered the electronic‐Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (e‐PPVT‐4), an electronic visual reasoning (VR) test, and a clinician‐administered Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI‐II). Probands also completed a full, in‐person, cognitive assessment and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition. Correlations between scores on electronic and clinician‐administered measures were examined. Classification accuracy of individual scores based on 95% confidence intervals and score range (below average, average, above average) were also assessed. Moderate to strong correlations were identified between both electronic measures and the clinician‐administered WASI‐II (ρ = 0.606–0.712). Mean difference between standard scores ranged from 10.7 to 14.8 for the cohort. Classification accuracy based on WASI‐II 95% confidence interval was consistently low (27.5%–47.3%). Classification accuracy by score range (below average, average, above average) was variable, ranging from 33% to 86% for probands. All participants unable to complete the electronic assessments met DSM‐5 criteria for intellectual disability. e‐PPVT‐4 and VR scores were strongly correlated with scores on the WASI‐II full‐scale IQ (ρ = 0.630, 0.712), indicating utility of these measures at the group level in large‐scale genomic studies. However, the poor precision of measurement across both measures suggests that the e‐PPVT‐4 and VR are not useful alternatives to in‐person testing for the purpose of clinical assessment of an individual's IQ score. LAY SUMMARY: Large‐scale studies designed to identify genes associated with autism have been successful in identifying over 100 genes. However, important clinical information about participants with autism and their family members is often missed—including cognitive functioning. Cognitive testing requires in‐person administration by a trained clinician and therefore can be burdensome and often reduces feasibility of diverse samples. Here, we assessed whether electronic assessments could take the place of in‐person cognitive testing. We found that at the group level, for large‐scale studies, electronic measures added valuable information; however, they were not accurate enough to be used on an individual level (i.e., to offer feedback about an individual's predicted IQ score). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 2022-03-12 2022-06 /pmc/articles/PMC9314947/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.2704 Text en © 2022 The Authors. Autism Research published by International Society for Autism Research and Wiley Periodicals LLC. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non‐commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
spellingShingle OMICS
Levy, Tess
Britvan, Bari
Grosman, Hannah
Giserman‐Kiss, Ivy
Meyering, Kristin
Weissman, Jordana
Halpern, Danielle
Zweifach, Jessica
Trelles, M. Pilar
Foss‐Feig, Jennifer H.
Kolevzon, Alexander
Sanders, Stephan J.
Robinson, Elise B.
Buxbaum, Joseph D.
Bishop, Somer
Siper, Paige M.
Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title_full Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title_fullStr Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title_full_unstemmed Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title_short Assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
title_sort assessing the utility of electronic measures as a proxy for cognitive ability
topic OMICS
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9314947/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.2704
work_keys_str_mv AT levytess assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT britvanbari assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT grosmanhannah assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT gisermankissivy assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT meyeringkristin assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT weissmanjordana assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT halperndanielle assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT zweifachjessica assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT trellesmpilar assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT fossfeigjenniferh assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT kolevzonalexander assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT sandersstephanj assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT robinsoneliseb assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT buxbaumjosephd assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT bishopsomer assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability
AT siperpaigem assessingtheutilityofelectronicmeasuresasaproxyforcognitiveability