Cargando…
Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities
BACKGROUND: Interrater reliability, aka intercoder reliability, is defined as true agreement between raters, aka coders, without chance agreement. It is used across many disciplines including medical and health research to measure the quality of ratings, coding, diagnoses, or other observations and...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
BioMed Central
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9426226/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36038846 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12874-022-01707-5 |
_version_ | 1784778637875085312 |
---|---|
author | Zhao, Xinshu Feng, Guangchao Charles Ao, Song Harris Liu, Piper Liping |
author_facet | Zhao, Xinshu Feng, Guangchao Charles Ao, Song Harris Liu, Piper Liping |
author_sort | Zhao, Xinshu |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: Interrater reliability, aka intercoder reliability, is defined as true agreement between raters, aka coders, without chance agreement. It is used across many disciplines including medical and health research to measure the quality of ratings, coding, diagnoses, or other observations and judgements. While numerous indices of interrater reliability are available, experts disagree on which ones are legitimate or more appropriate. Almost all agree that percent agreement (a(o)), the oldest and the simplest index, is also the most flawed because it fails to estimate and remove chance agreement, which is produced by raters’ random rating. The experts, however, disagree on which chance estimators are legitimate or better. The experts also disagree on which of the three factors, rating category, distribution skew, or task difficulty, an index should rely on to estimate chance agreement, or which factors the known indices in fact rely on. The most popular chance-adjusted indices, according to a functionalist view of mathematical statistics, assume that all raters conduct intentional and maximum random rating while typical raters conduct involuntary and reluctant random rating. The mismatches between the assumed and the actual rater behaviors cause the indices to rely on mistaken factors to estimate chance agreement, leading to the numerous paradoxes, abnormalities, and other misbehaviors of the indices identified by prior studies. METHODS: We conducted a 4 × 8 × 3 between-subject controlled experiment with 4 subjects per cell. Each subject was a rating session with 100 pairs of rating by two raters, totaling 384 rating sessions as the experimental subjects. The experiment tested seven best-known indices of interrater reliability against the observed reliabilities and chance agreements. Impacts of the three factors, i.e., rating category, distribution skew, and task difficulty, on the indices were tested. RESULTS: The most criticized index, percent agreement (a(o)), showed as the most accurate predictor of reliability, reporting directional r(2) = .84. It was also the third best approximator, overestimating observed reliability by 13 percentage points on average. The three most acclaimed and most popular indices, Scott’s π, Cohen’s κ and Krippendorff’s α, underperformed all other indices, reporting directional r(2) = .312 and underestimated reliability by 31.4 ~ 31.8 points. The newest index, Gwet’s AC(1), emerged as the second-best predictor and the most accurate approximator. Bennett et al’s S ranked behind AC(1), and Perreault and Leigh’s I(r) ranked the fourth both for prediction and approximation. The reliance on category and skew and failure to rely on difficulty explain why the six chance-adjusted indices often underperformed a(o), which they were created to outperform. The evidence corroborated the notion that the chance-adjusted indices assume intentional and maximum random rating while the raters instead exhibited involuntary and reluctant random rating. CONCLUSION: The authors call for more empirical studies and especially more controlled experiments to falsify or qualify this study. If the main findings are replicated and the underlying theories supported, new thinking and new indices may be needed. Index designers may need to refrain from assuming intentional and maximum random rating, and instead assume involuntary and reluctant random rating. Accordingly, the new indices may need to rely on task difficulty, rather than distribution skew or rating category, to estimate chance agreement. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12874-022-01707-5. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9426226 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | BioMed Central |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-94262262022-08-31 Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities Zhao, Xinshu Feng, Guangchao Charles Ao, Song Harris Liu, Piper Liping BMC Med Res Methodol Research BACKGROUND: Interrater reliability, aka intercoder reliability, is defined as true agreement between raters, aka coders, without chance agreement. It is used across many disciplines including medical and health research to measure the quality of ratings, coding, diagnoses, or other observations and judgements. While numerous indices of interrater reliability are available, experts disagree on which ones are legitimate or more appropriate. Almost all agree that percent agreement (a(o)), the oldest and the simplest index, is also the most flawed because it fails to estimate and remove chance agreement, which is produced by raters’ random rating. The experts, however, disagree on which chance estimators are legitimate or better. The experts also disagree on which of the three factors, rating category, distribution skew, or task difficulty, an index should rely on to estimate chance agreement, or which factors the known indices in fact rely on. The most popular chance-adjusted indices, according to a functionalist view of mathematical statistics, assume that all raters conduct intentional and maximum random rating while typical raters conduct involuntary and reluctant random rating. The mismatches between the assumed and the actual rater behaviors cause the indices to rely on mistaken factors to estimate chance agreement, leading to the numerous paradoxes, abnormalities, and other misbehaviors of the indices identified by prior studies. METHODS: We conducted a 4 × 8 × 3 between-subject controlled experiment with 4 subjects per cell. Each subject was a rating session with 100 pairs of rating by two raters, totaling 384 rating sessions as the experimental subjects. The experiment tested seven best-known indices of interrater reliability against the observed reliabilities and chance agreements. Impacts of the three factors, i.e., rating category, distribution skew, and task difficulty, on the indices were tested. RESULTS: The most criticized index, percent agreement (a(o)), showed as the most accurate predictor of reliability, reporting directional r(2) = .84. It was also the third best approximator, overestimating observed reliability by 13 percentage points on average. The three most acclaimed and most popular indices, Scott’s π, Cohen’s κ and Krippendorff’s α, underperformed all other indices, reporting directional r(2) = .312 and underestimated reliability by 31.4 ~ 31.8 points. The newest index, Gwet’s AC(1), emerged as the second-best predictor and the most accurate approximator. Bennett et al’s S ranked behind AC(1), and Perreault and Leigh’s I(r) ranked the fourth both for prediction and approximation. The reliance on category and skew and failure to rely on difficulty explain why the six chance-adjusted indices often underperformed a(o), which they were created to outperform. The evidence corroborated the notion that the chance-adjusted indices assume intentional and maximum random rating while the raters instead exhibited involuntary and reluctant random rating. CONCLUSION: The authors call for more empirical studies and especially more controlled experiments to falsify or qualify this study. If the main findings are replicated and the underlying theories supported, new thinking and new indices may be needed. Index designers may need to refrain from assuming intentional and maximum random rating, and instead assume involuntary and reluctant random rating. Accordingly, the new indices may need to rely on task difficulty, rather than distribution skew or rating category, to estimate chance agreement. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s12874-022-01707-5. BioMed Central 2022-08-29 /pmc/articles/PMC9426226/ /pubmed/36038846 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12874-022-01707-5 Text en © The Author(s) 2022 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data. |
spellingShingle | Research Zhao, Xinshu Feng, Guangchao Charles Ao, Song Harris Liu, Piper Liping Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title | Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title_full | Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title_fullStr | Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title_full_unstemmed | Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title_short | Interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
title_sort | interrater reliability estimators tested against true interrater reliabilities |
topic | Research |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9426226/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36038846 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12874-022-01707-5 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT zhaoxinshu interraterreliabilityestimatorstestedagainsttrueinterraterreliabilities AT fengguangchaocharles interraterreliabilityestimatorstestedagainsttrueinterraterreliabilities AT aosongharris interraterreliabilityestimatorstestedagainsttrueinterraterreliabilities AT liupiperliping interraterreliabilityestimatorstestedagainsttrueinterraterreliabilities |