Cargando…
The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content
BACKGROUND: Studies on person perception showed that stereotypes can be activated by presenting either characteristic traits of group members, or labels associated to these groups. However, it is not clear whether these pieces of semantic information activate negative and positive stereotypes direct...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
BioMed Central
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9494850/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36131295 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00928-z |
_version_ | 1784793881032785920 |
---|---|
author | Kocsor, Ferenc Ferencz, Tas Kisander, Zsolt Tizedes, Gitta Schaadt, Blanka Kertész, Rita Kozma, Luca Vincze, Orsolya Láng, András |
author_facet | Kocsor, Ferenc Ferencz, Tas Kisander, Zsolt Tizedes, Gitta Schaadt, Blanka Kertész, Rita Kozma, Luca Vincze, Orsolya Láng, András |
author_sort | Kocsor, Ferenc |
collection | PubMed |
description | BACKGROUND: Studies on person perception showed that stereotypes can be activated by presenting either characteristic traits of group members, or labels associated to these groups. However, it is not clear whether these pieces of semantic information activate negative and positive stereotypes directly, or via an indirect cognitive pathway leading through brain regions responsible for affective responses. Our main objective with this study was to disentangle the effects of semantic and affective contents. To this end, we intended to scrutinize whether the representation of occupational labels is independent of the emotions they evoke. METHODS: Participants (N = 73, M = 27.0, SD = 9.1, 31 men 42 women,) were asked to complete two tasks presented online. In the first task they had to arrange 20 occupational labels—randomly chosen from a pool of 60 items—in a two-dimensional space, moving the mouse pointer along two undefined axes. In a second task the axes’ names were defined a priori. Subjects were asked to arrange the labels according to valence, the extent to which the word evoked pleasant or unpleasant feelings, and arousal, the extent to which the word evoked excitement or calmness. RESULTS: Based on the final coordinates of the labels, two cluster analyses were carried out separately in the two tasks. The two clusters were compared with Fisher’s exact test, which revealed that the cluster structures overlap significantly. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that the spontaneous categorization and the semantic representation of occupations rely largely on the affective state they evoke. We propose that affective content might have a primacy over detailed semantic information in many aspects of person perception, including social categorization. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40359-022-00928-z. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9494850 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | BioMed Central |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-94948502022-09-23 The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content Kocsor, Ferenc Ferencz, Tas Kisander, Zsolt Tizedes, Gitta Schaadt, Blanka Kertész, Rita Kozma, Luca Vincze, Orsolya Láng, András BMC Psychol Research BACKGROUND: Studies on person perception showed that stereotypes can be activated by presenting either characteristic traits of group members, or labels associated to these groups. However, it is not clear whether these pieces of semantic information activate negative and positive stereotypes directly, or via an indirect cognitive pathway leading through brain regions responsible for affective responses. Our main objective with this study was to disentangle the effects of semantic and affective contents. To this end, we intended to scrutinize whether the representation of occupational labels is independent of the emotions they evoke. METHODS: Participants (N = 73, M = 27.0, SD = 9.1, 31 men 42 women,) were asked to complete two tasks presented online. In the first task they had to arrange 20 occupational labels—randomly chosen from a pool of 60 items—in a two-dimensional space, moving the mouse pointer along two undefined axes. In a second task the axes’ names were defined a priori. Subjects were asked to arrange the labels according to valence, the extent to which the word evoked pleasant or unpleasant feelings, and arousal, the extent to which the word evoked excitement or calmness. RESULTS: Based on the final coordinates of the labels, two cluster analyses were carried out separately in the two tasks. The two clusters were compared with Fisher’s exact test, which revealed that the cluster structures overlap significantly. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that the spontaneous categorization and the semantic representation of occupations rely largely on the affective state they evoke. We propose that affective content might have a primacy over detailed semantic information in many aspects of person perception, including social categorization. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40359-022-00928-z. BioMed Central 2022-09-21 /pmc/articles/PMC9494850/ /pubmed/36131295 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00928-z Text en © The Author(s) 2022 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data. |
spellingShingle | Research Kocsor, Ferenc Ferencz, Tas Kisander, Zsolt Tizedes, Gitta Schaadt, Blanka Kertész, Rita Kozma, Luca Vincze, Orsolya Láng, András The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title | The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title_full | The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title_fullStr | The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title_full_unstemmed | The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title_short | The mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
title_sort | mental representation of occupational stereotypes is driven as much by their affective as by their semantic content |
topic | Research |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9494850/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36131295 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00928-z |
work_keys_str_mv | AT kocsorferenc thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT ferencztas thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kisanderzsolt thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT tizedesgitta thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT schaadtblanka thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kerteszrita thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kozmaluca thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT vinczeorsolya thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT langandras thementalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kocsorferenc mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT ferencztas mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kisanderzsolt mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT tizedesgitta mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT schaadtblanka mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kerteszrita mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT kozmaluca mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT vinczeorsolya mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent AT langandras mentalrepresentationofoccupationalstereotypesisdrivenasmuchbytheiraffectiveasbytheirsemanticcontent |