Cargando…
3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context
Imitation provides a reliable method to investigate the developing memory functions in childhood. The present study explored whether 3-4-year-old children are able to revise their previous experiences after a 1 week delay in order to adapt to an altered context. We used a combined short-term (Sessio...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9506616/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36149884 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275071 |
_version_ | 1784796767133368320 |
---|---|
author | Liszkai-Peres, Krisztina Kampis, Dora Király, Ildikó |
author_facet | Liszkai-Peres, Krisztina Kampis, Dora Király, Ildikó |
author_sort | Liszkai-Peres, Krisztina |
collection | PubMed |
description | Imitation provides a reliable method to investigate the developing memory functions in childhood. The present study explored whether 3-4-year-old children are able to revise their previous experiences after a 1 week delay in order to adapt to an altered context. We used a combined short-term (Session 1) and delayed (Session 2) imitation paradigm based on a previous study with 2-year-olds. The constraints (target object close/far) and relatedly the relevance of using a tool in a goal attainment task (irrelevant/relevant, respectively) changed between the sessions. We found that children in Session 1 used the tool only when it was needed (relevant/object far context). After the 1 week delay when the tool was previously irrelevant and then became relevant, children remembered the irrelevant act and applied it in the altered context. When the tool lost its relevance after 1 week, children used the tool less than before, but did not fully omit it, despite its reduced efficiency. The present data with 3-year-olds was compared to a pattern of results with 2-year-olds (from a similar previous study), that allowed to discuss possible developmental transitions in memory and imitation. We propose that the flexible restoration of a formerly irrelevant act and the maintenance of a formerly successful solution indicate flexibility of preschooler’s memory when guiding imitation. This flexibility, however, interacts with children’s tendency to remain faithful to strategies that were previously ostensively demonstrated to them. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9506616 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | Public Library of Science |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-95066162022-09-24 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context Liszkai-Peres, Krisztina Kampis, Dora Király, Ildikó PLoS One Research Article Imitation provides a reliable method to investigate the developing memory functions in childhood. The present study explored whether 3-4-year-old children are able to revise their previous experiences after a 1 week delay in order to adapt to an altered context. We used a combined short-term (Session 1) and delayed (Session 2) imitation paradigm based on a previous study with 2-year-olds. The constraints (target object close/far) and relatedly the relevance of using a tool in a goal attainment task (irrelevant/relevant, respectively) changed between the sessions. We found that children in Session 1 used the tool only when it was needed (relevant/object far context). After the 1 week delay when the tool was previously irrelevant and then became relevant, children remembered the irrelevant act and applied it in the altered context. When the tool lost its relevance after 1 week, children used the tool less than before, but did not fully omit it, despite its reduced efficiency. The present data with 3-year-olds was compared to a pattern of results with 2-year-olds (from a similar previous study), that allowed to discuss possible developmental transitions in memory and imitation. We propose that the flexible restoration of a formerly irrelevant act and the maintenance of a formerly successful solution indicate flexibility of preschooler’s memory when guiding imitation. This flexibility, however, interacts with children’s tendency to remain faithful to strategies that were previously ostensively demonstrated to them. Public Library of Science 2022-09-23 /pmc/articles/PMC9506616/ /pubmed/36149884 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275071 Text en © 2022 Liszkai-Peres et al https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. |
spellingShingle | Research Article Liszkai-Peres, Krisztina Kampis, Dora Király, Ildikó 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title_full | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title_fullStr | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title_full_unstemmed | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title_short | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
title_sort | 3-4-year-old children’s memory flexibility allows adaptation to an altered context |
topic | Research Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9506616/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36149884 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0275071 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT liszkaipereskrisztina 34yearoldchildrensmemoryflexibilityallowsadaptationtoanalteredcontext AT kampisdora 34yearoldchildrensmemoryflexibilityallowsadaptationtoanalteredcontext AT kiralyildiko 34yearoldchildrensmemoryflexibilityallowsadaptationtoanalteredcontext |