Cargando…
The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence
Despite the importance of self‐control for well‐being and adjustment, its development from early childhood to early adolescence has been relatively understudied. We addressed the development of mother‐reported self‐control in what is likely the largest and longest longitudinal twin study of the topi...
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
John Wiley and Sons Inc.
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9539564/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35436381 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13270 |
_version_ | 1784803516454273024 |
---|---|
author | Pener‐Tessler, Roni Markovitch, Noam Knafo‐Noam, Ariel |
author_facet | Pener‐Tessler, Roni Markovitch, Noam Knafo‐Noam, Ariel |
author_sort | Pener‐Tessler, Roni |
collection | PubMed |
description | Despite the importance of self‐control for well‐being and adjustment, its development from early childhood to early adolescence has been relatively understudied. We addressed the development of mother‐reported self‐control in what is likely the largest and longest longitudinal twin study of the topic to this day (N = 1889 individual children with data from at least one of five waves: ages 3, 5, 6.5, 8–9 and 11 years). We examined rank‐order change in self‐control from early childhood to early adolescence, genetic and environmental contributions to variance in the trait and differential developmental trajectories. The relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to change and stability was also examined. Results point at middle childhood as a period of potential transition and change. During this period the rank‐order stability of self‐control increases, heritability rates substantially rise, and a cross‐over occurs in two of the self‐control trajectories. Nonadditive genetic effects contribute to both stability and change in self‐control while the nonshared environment contributes mostly to change, with no effect for the shared environment. Our findings suggest that new genetic factors, that emerge around age 6.5 and whose effect on self‐control is carried on along development, may partially account for changes in self‐control around late middle childhood, and explain the growing stability in the trait approaching early adolescence. We discuss the implications of the special role of middle childhood for self‐control development. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9539564 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | John Wiley and Sons Inc. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-95395642022-10-14 The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence Pener‐Tessler, Roni Markovitch, Noam Knafo‐Noam, Ariel Dev Sci Research Articles Despite the importance of self‐control for well‐being and adjustment, its development from early childhood to early adolescence has been relatively understudied. We addressed the development of mother‐reported self‐control in what is likely the largest and longest longitudinal twin study of the topic to this day (N = 1889 individual children with data from at least one of five waves: ages 3, 5, 6.5, 8–9 and 11 years). We examined rank‐order change in self‐control from early childhood to early adolescence, genetic and environmental contributions to variance in the trait and differential developmental trajectories. The relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to change and stability was also examined. Results point at middle childhood as a period of potential transition and change. During this period the rank‐order stability of self‐control increases, heritability rates substantially rise, and a cross‐over occurs in two of the self‐control trajectories. Nonadditive genetic effects contribute to both stability and change in self‐control while the nonshared environment contributes mostly to change, with no effect for the shared environment. Our findings suggest that new genetic factors, that emerge around age 6.5 and whose effect on self‐control is carried on along development, may partially account for changes in self‐control around late middle childhood, and explain the growing stability in the trait approaching early adolescence. We discuss the implications of the special role of middle childhood for self‐control development. John Wiley and Sons Inc. 2022-05-24 2022-09 /pmc/articles/PMC9539564/ /pubmed/35436381 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13270 Text en © 2022 The Authors. Developmental Science published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non‐commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made. |
spellingShingle | Research Articles Pener‐Tessler, Roni Markovitch, Noam Knafo‐Noam, Ariel The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title | The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title_full | The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title_fullStr | The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title_full_unstemmed | The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title_short | The special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: Longitudinal and genetic evidence |
title_sort | special role of middle childhood in self‐control development: longitudinal and genetic evidence |
topic | Research Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9539564/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35436381 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.13270 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT penertesslerroni thespecialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence AT markovitchnoam thespecialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence AT knafonoamariel thespecialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence AT penertesslerroni specialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence AT markovitchnoam specialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence AT knafonoamariel specialroleofmiddlechildhoodinselfcontroldevelopmentlongitudinalandgeneticevidence |