Cargando…
Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study
INTRODUCTION: Physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep are associated with cognitive function in older adults. However, these behaviours are not independent, but instead make up exclusive and exhaustive components of the 24-h day. Few studies have investigated associations between 24-h time-...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9729737/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36504624 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.1051793 |
_version_ | 1784845533563584512 |
---|---|
author | Mellow, Maddison L. Dumuid, Dorothea Wade, Alexandra T. Stanford, Ty Olds, Timothy S. Karayanidis, Frini Hunter, Montana Keage, Hannah A. D. Dorrian, Jillian Goldsworthy, Mitchell R. Smith, Ashleigh E. |
author_facet | Mellow, Maddison L. Dumuid, Dorothea Wade, Alexandra T. Stanford, Ty Olds, Timothy S. Karayanidis, Frini Hunter, Montana Keage, Hannah A. D. Dorrian, Jillian Goldsworthy, Mitchell R. Smith, Ashleigh E. |
author_sort | Mellow, Maddison L. |
collection | PubMed |
description | INTRODUCTION: Physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep are associated with cognitive function in older adults. However, these behaviours are not independent, but instead make up exclusive and exhaustive components of the 24-h day. Few studies have investigated associations between 24-h time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults. Of these, none have considered how the quality of sleep, or the context of physical activity and sedentary behaviour may impact these relationships. This study aims to understand how 24-h time-use composition is associated with cognitive function across a range of domains in healthy older adults, and whether the level of recreational physical activity, amount of television (TV) watching, or the quality of sleep impact these potential associations. METHODS: 384 healthy older adults (age 65.5 ± 3.0 years, 68% female, 63% non-smokers, mean education = 16.5 ± 3.2 years) participated in this study across two Australian sites (Adelaide, n = 207; Newcastle, n = 177). Twenty-four-hour time-use composition was captured using triaxial accelerometry, measured continuously across 7 days. Total time spent watching TV per day was used to capture the context of sedentary behaviours, whilst total time spent in recreational physical activity was used to capture the context of physical activity (i.e., recreational accumulation of physical activity vs. other contexts). Sleep quality was measured using a single item extracted from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Cognitive function was measured using a global cognition index (Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III) and four cognitive domain composite scores (derived from five tests of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery: Paired Associates Learning; One Touch Stockings of Cambridge; Multitasking; Reaction Time; Verbal Recognition Memory). Pairwise correlations were used to describe independent relationships between time use variables and cognitive outcomes. Then, compositional data analysis regression methods were used to quantify associations between cognition and 24-h time-use composition. RESULTS: After adjusting for covariates and false discovery rate there were no significant associations between time-use composition and global cognition, long-term memory, short-term memory, executive function, or processing speed outcomes, and no significant interactions between TV watching time, recreational physical activity engagement or sleep quality and time-use composition for any cognitive outcomes. DISCUSSION: The findings highlight the importance of considering all activities across the 24-h day against cognitive function in older adults. Future studies should consider investigating these relationships longitudinally to uncover temporal effects. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9729737 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-97297372022-12-09 Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study Mellow, Maddison L. Dumuid, Dorothea Wade, Alexandra T. Stanford, Ty Olds, Timothy S. Karayanidis, Frini Hunter, Montana Keage, Hannah A. D. Dorrian, Jillian Goldsworthy, Mitchell R. Smith, Ashleigh E. Front Hum Neurosci Neuroscience INTRODUCTION: Physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep are associated with cognitive function in older adults. However, these behaviours are not independent, but instead make up exclusive and exhaustive components of the 24-h day. Few studies have investigated associations between 24-h time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults. Of these, none have considered how the quality of sleep, or the context of physical activity and sedentary behaviour may impact these relationships. This study aims to understand how 24-h time-use composition is associated with cognitive function across a range of domains in healthy older adults, and whether the level of recreational physical activity, amount of television (TV) watching, or the quality of sleep impact these potential associations. METHODS: 384 healthy older adults (age 65.5 ± 3.0 years, 68% female, 63% non-smokers, mean education = 16.5 ± 3.2 years) participated in this study across two Australian sites (Adelaide, n = 207; Newcastle, n = 177). Twenty-four-hour time-use composition was captured using triaxial accelerometry, measured continuously across 7 days. Total time spent watching TV per day was used to capture the context of sedentary behaviours, whilst total time spent in recreational physical activity was used to capture the context of physical activity (i.e., recreational accumulation of physical activity vs. other contexts). Sleep quality was measured using a single item extracted from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Cognitive function was measured using a global cognition index (Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III) and four cognitive domain composite scores (derived from five tests of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery: Paired Associates Learning; One Touch Stockings of Cambridge; Multitasking; Reaction Time; Verbal Recognition Memory). Pairwise correlations were used to describe independent relationships between time use variables and cognitive outcomes. Then, compositional data analysis regression methods were used to quantify associations between cognition and 24-h time-use composition. RESULTS: After adjusting for covariates and false discovery rate there were no significant associations between time-use composition and global cognition, long-term memory, short-term memory, executive function, or processing speed outcomes, and no significant interactions between TV watching time, recreational physical activity engagement or sleep quality and time-use composition for any cognitive outcomes. DISCUSSION: The findings highlight the importance of considering all activities across the 24-h day against cognitive function in older adults. Future studies should consider investigating these relationships longitudinally to uncover temporal effects. Frontiers Media S.A. 2022-11-24 /pmc/articles/PMC9729737/ /pubmed/36504624 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.1051793 Text en Copyright © 2022 Mellow, Dumuid, Wade, Stanford, Olds, Karayanidis, Hunter, Keage, Dorrian, Goldsworthy and Smith. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Neuroscience Mellow, Maddison L. Dumuid, Dorothea Wade, Alexandra T. Stanford, Ty Olds, Timothy S. Karayanidis, Frini Hunter, Montana Keage, Hannah A. D. Dorrian, Jillian Goldsworthy, Mitchell R. Smith, Ashleigh E. Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title | Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title_full | Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title_fullStr | Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title_full_unstemmed | Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title_short | Twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the ACTIVate study |
title_sort | twenty-four-hour time-use composition and cognitive function in older adults: cross-sectional findings of the activate study |
topic | Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9729737/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36504624 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.1051793 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT mellowmaddisonl twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT dumuiddorothea twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT wadealexandrat twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT stanfordty twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT oldstimothys twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT karayanidisfrini twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT huntermontana twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT keagehannahad twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT dorrianjillian twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT goldsworthymitchellr twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy AT smithashleighe twentyfourhourtimeusecompositionandcognitivefunctioninolderadultscrosssectionalfindingsoftheactivatestudy |