Cargando…

Children's strategies in drop-landing

INTRODUCTION: Landing is a critical motor skill included in many activities performed in the natural environment by young children. Yet, landing is critically relevance to ensure proper stability and reduce injury. Furthermore, landing is an integral part of many fundamental motor skills which have...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Angulo-Barroso, Rosa, Ferrer-Uris, Blai, Jubany, Júlia, Busquets, Albert
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9755734/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36532989
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.982467
_version_ 1784851482922713088
author Angulo-Barroso, Rosa
Ferrer-Uris, Blai
Jubany, Júlia
Busquets, Albert
author_facet Angulo-Barroso, Rosa
Ferrer-Uris, Blai
Jubany, Júlia
Busquets, Albert
author_sort Angulo-Barroso, Rosa
collection PubMed
description INTRODUCTION: Landing is a critical motor skill included in many activities performed in the natural environment by young children. Yet, landing is critically relevance to ensure proper stability and reduce injury. Furthermore, landing is an integral part of many fundamental motor skills which have been linked to greater physical activity, sport participation, and perceived competence in children. Our aim was to examine the drop-landing strategies of young children focusing on the lower extremity with a multi-variant approach. METHODS: Forty-four children divided into four age groups (G1:3–4.5 y, G2:4.5–6 y, G3:6–7.5 y, G4:7.5–9 y) performed 20 drop-land trials in four different conditions: predictable stationary landing, running to the left, to the right, and stay in place. Fifteen reflective markers, two force plates, and ten surface electromyography (sEMG) sensors were used to collect data. MANOVAs (Group x Condition) were conducted separately for the kinematic, kinetic, and sEMG variables. RESULTS: Only significant group effects were found (kinematic MANOVA p = 0.039, kinetic MANOVA p = 0.007, and sEMG MANOVA p = 0.012), suggesting that younger groups (G1, G2) differed to the older groups (G3, G4). G1 showed less knee flexion and slower ankle dorsi-flexion during the braking phase compared to G3, while G2 presented smaller ankle dorsi-flexion at the braking phase and smaller ankle range of motion than G3. Overall kinetic variables analysis showed a group difference but no group differences for any single kinetic variable alone was found. Regarding sEMG, G1 during the flight phase exhibited longer tibialis anterior and hamstrings activity than G3 and G3 & G4, respectively; and an earlier start of the hamstrings' impact burst than G4. In addition, distal to proximal control was primarily used by all groups to coordinate muscle activity (in response to impact) and joint motion (after impact). DISCUSSION: Perhaps a developmental critical point in landing performance exists at 4–5 years of age since G1 presented the largest differences among the groups. This suggests that to improve landing strategies could start around this age. Future studies should examine if playground environments that include equipment conducive to landing and practitioners in the kindergarten schools are adequate vehicles to empower this type of intervention.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9755734
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-97557342022-12-17 Children's strategies in drop-landing Angulo-Barroso, Rosa Ferrer-Uris, Blai Jubany, Júlia Busquets, Albert Front Psychol Psychology INTRODUCTION: Landing is a critical motor skill included in many activities performed in the natural environment by young children. Yet, landing is critically relevance to ensure proper stability and reduce injury. Furthermore, landing is an integral part of many fundamental motor skills which have been linked to greater physical activity, sport participation, and perceived competence in children. Our aim was to examine the drop-landing strategies of young children focusing on the lower extremity with a multi-variant approach. METHODS: Forty-four children divided into four age groups (G1:3–4.5 y, G2:4.5–6 y, G3:6–7.5 y, G4:7.5–9 y) performed 20 drop-land trials in four different conditions: predictable stationary landing, running to the left, to the right, and stay in place. Fifteen reflective markers, two force plates, and ten surface electromyography (sEMG) sensors were used to collect data. MANOVAs (Group x Condition) were conducted separately for the kinematic, kinetic, and sEMG variables. RESULTS: Only significant group effects were found (kinematic MANOVA p = 0.039, kinetic MANOVA p = 0.007, and sEMG MANOVA p = 0.012), suggesting that younger groups (G1, G2) differed to the older groups (G3, G4). G1 showed less knee flexion and slower ankle dorsi-flexion during the braking phase compared to G3, while G2 presented smaller ankle dorsi-flexion at the braking phase and smaller ankle range of motion than G3. Overall kinetic variables analysis showed a group difference but no group differences for any single kinetic variable alone was found. Regarding sEMG, G1 during the flight phase exhibited longer tibialis anterior and hamstrings activity than G3 and G3 & G4, respectively; and an earlier start of the hamstrings' impact burst than G4. In addition, distal to proximal control was primarily used by all groups to coordinate muscle activity (in response to impact) and joint motion (after impact). DISCUSSION: Perhaps a developmental critical point in landing performance exists at 4–5 years of age since G1 presented the largest differences among the groups. This suggests that to improve landing strategies could start around this age. Future studies should examine if playground environments that include equipment conducive to landing and practitioners in the kindergarten schools are adequate vehicles to empower this type of intervention. Frontiers Media S.A. 2022-12-02 /pmc/articles/PMC9755734/ /pubmed/36532989 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.982467 Text en Copyright © 2022 Angulo-Barroso, Ferrer-Uris, Jubany and Busquets. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Angulo-Barroso, Rosa
Ferrer-Uris, Blai
Jubany, Júlia
Busquets, Albert
Children's strategies in drop-landing
title Children's strategies in drop-landing
title_full Children's strategies in drop-landing
title_fullStr Children's strategies in drop-landing
title_full_unstemmed Children's strategies in drop-landing
title_short Children's strategies in drop-landing
title_sort children's strategies in drop-landing
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9755734/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36532989
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.982467
work_keys_str_mv AT angulobarrosorosa childrensstrategiesindroplanding
AT ferrerurisblai childrensstrategiesindroplanding
AT jubanyjulia childrensstrategiesindroplanding
AT busquetsalbert childrensstrategiesindroplanding