Cargando…

Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis

Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ra...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Zhang, Hanshu, Hung, Shen-Wu, Chen, Yu-Pin, Ku, Jan-Wen, Tseng, Philip, Lu, Yueh-Hsun, Yang, Cheng-Ta
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Springer International Publishing 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9813289/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36600082
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w
_version_ 1784863896186650624
author Zhang, Hanshu
Hung, Shen-Wu
Chen, Yu-Pin
Ku, Jan-Wen
Tseng, Philip
Lu, Yueh-Hsun
Yang, Cheng-Ta
author_facet Zhang, Hanshu
Hung, Shen-Wu
Chen, Yu-Pin
Ku, Jan-Wen
Tseng, Philip
Lu, Yueh-Hsun
Yang, Cheng-Ta
author_sort Zhang, Hanshu
collection PubMed
description Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ray images with either high prevalence (N(signal) = 40) or low prevalence (N(signal) = 10). Results showed that compared to novices (e.g., pediatricians, dentists, neurologists), the manipulation of prevalence shifted participant’s criteria in a different direction for experts who perform hip fracture diagnosis on a daily basis. That is, when prevalence rate is low (p(fracture-present) = 0.2), experts held more conservative criteria in answering “fracture-present,” whereas novices were more likely to believe there was fracture. Importantly, participants’ detection discriminability did not vary by the prevalence condition. In addition, all participants were more conservative with “fracture-present” responses when task difficulty increased. We suspect the apparent opposite criteria shift between experts and novices may have come from medical training that made novices to believe that a miss would result in larger cost compared to false positive, or because they failed to update their prior belief about the signal prevalence in the task, both would suggest that novices and experts may have different beliefs in placing the optimal strategy in the hip fracture diagnosis. Our work can contribute to medical education training as well as other applied clinical diagnosis that aims to mitigate the prevalence effect. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9813289
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher Springer International Publishing
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-98132892023-01-06 Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis Zhang, Hanshu Hung, Shen-Wu Chen, Yu-Pin Ku, Jan-Wen Tseng, Philip Lu, Yueh-Hsun Yang, Cheng-Ta Cogn Res Princ Implic Original Article Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ray images with either high prevalence (N(signal) = 40) or low prevalence (N(signal) = 10). Results showed that compared to novices (e.g., pediatricians, dentists, neurologists), the manipulation of prevalence shifted participant’s criteria in a different direction for experts who perform hip fracture diagnosis on a daily basis. That is, when prevalence rate is low (p(fracture-present) = 0.2), experts held more conservative criteria in answering “fracture-present,” whereas novices were more likely to believe there was fracture. Importantly, participants’ detection discriminability did not vary by the prevalence condition. In addition, all participants were more conservative with “fracture-present” responses when task difficulty increased. We suspect the apparent opposite criteria shift between experts and novices may have come from medical training that made novices to believe that a miss would result in larger cost compared to false positive, or because they failed to update their prior belief about the signal prevalence in the task, both would suggest that novices and experts may have different beliefs in placing the optimal strategy in the hip fracture diagnosis. Our work can contribute to medical education training as well as other applied clinical diagnosis that aims to mitigate the prevalence effect. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w. Springer International Publishing 2023-01-05 /pmc/articles/PMC9813289/ /pubmed/36600082 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w Text en © The Author(s) 2022 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Original Article
Zhang, Hanshu
Hung, Shen-Wu
Chen, Yu-Pin
Ku, Jan-Wen
Tseng, Philip
Lu, Yueh-Hsun
Yang, Cheng-Ta
Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title_full Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title_fullStr Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title_full_unstemmed Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title_short Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
title_sort hip fracture or not? the reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
topic Original Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9813289/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36600082
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w
work_keys_str_mv AT zhanghanshu hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT hungshenwu hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT chenyupin hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT kujanwen hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT tsengphilip hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT luyuehhsun hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis
AT yangchengta hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis