Cargando…
Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis
Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ra...
Autores principales: | , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer International Publishing
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9813289/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36600082 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w |
_version_ | 1784863896186650624 |
---|---|
author | Zhang, Hanshu Hung, Shen-Wu Chen, Yu-Pin Ku, Jan-Wen Tseng, Philip Lu, Yueh-Hsun Yang, Cheng-Ta |
author_facet | Zhang, Hanshu Hung, Shen-Wu Chen, Yu-Pin Ku, Jan-Wen Tseng, Philip Lu, Yueh-Hsun Yang, Cheng-Ta |
author_sort | Zhang, Hanshu |
collection | PubMed |
description | Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ray images with either high prevalence (N(signal) = 40) or low prevalence (N(signal) = 10). Results showed that compared to novices (e.g., pediatricians, dentists, neurologists), the manipulation of prevalence shifted participant’s criteria in a different direction for experts who perform hip fracture diagnosis on a daily basis. That is, when prevalence rate is low (p(fracture-present) = 0.2), experts held more conservative criteria in answering “fracture-present,” whereas novices were more likely to believe there was fracture. Importantly, participants’ detection discriminability did not vary by the prevalence condition. In addition, all participants were more conservative with “fracture-present” responses when task difficulty increased. We suspect the apparent opposite criteria shift between experts and novices may have come from medical training that made novices to believe that a miss would result in larger cost compared to false positive, or because they failed to update their prior belief about the signal prevalence in the task, both would suggest that novices and experts may have different beliefs in placing the optimal strategy in the hip fracture diagnosis. Our work can contribute to medical education training as well as other applied clinical diagnosis that aims to mitigate the prevalence effect. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9813289 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | Springer International Publishing |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-98132892023-01-06 Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis Zhang, Hanshu Hung, Shen-Wu Chen, Yu-Pin Ku, Jan-Wen Tseng, Philip Lu, Yueh-Hsun Yang, Cheng-Ta Cogn Res Princ Implic Original Article Despite numerous investigations of the prevalence effect on medical image perception, little research has been done to examine the effect of expertise, and its possible interaction with prevalence. In this study, medical practitioners were instructed to detect the presence of hip fracture in 50 X-ray images with either high prevalence (N(signal) = 40) or low prevalence (N(signal) = 10). Results showed that compared to novices (e.g., pediatricians, dentists, neurologists), the manipulation of prevalence shifted participant’s criteria in a different direction for experts who perform hip fracture diagnosis on a daily basis. That is, when prevalence rate is low (p(fracture-present) = 0.2), experts held more conservative criteria in answering “fracture-present,” whereas novices were more likely to believe there was fracture. Importantly, participants’ detection discriminability did not vary by the prevalence condition. In addition, all participants were more conservative with “fracture-present” responses when task difficulty increased. We suspect the apparent opposite criteria shift between experts and novices may have come from medical training that made novices to believe that a miss would result in larger cost compared to false positive, or because they failed to update their prior belief about the signal prevalence in the task, both would suggest that novices and experts may have different beliefs in placing the optimal strategy in the hip fracture diagnosis. Our work can contribute to medical education training as well as other applied clinical diagnosis that aims to mitigate the prevalence effect. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w. Springer International Publishing 2023-01-05 /pmc/articles/PMC9813289/ /pubmed/36600082 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w Text en © The Author(s) 2022 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) . |
spellingShingle | Original Article Zhang, Hanshu Hung, Shen-Wu Chen, Yu-Pin Ku, Jan-Wen Tseng, Philip Lu, Yueh-Hsun Yang, Cheng-Ta Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title | Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title_full | Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title_fullStr | Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title_full_unstemmed | Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title_short | Hip fracture or not? The reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
title_sort | hip fracture or not? the reversed prevalence effect among non-experts’ diagnosis |
topic | Original Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9813289/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36600082 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s41235-022-00455-w |
work_keys_str_mv | AT zhanghanshu hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT hungshenwu hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT chenyupin hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT kujanwen hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT tsengphilip hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT luyuehhsun hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis AT yangchengta hipfractureornotthereversedprevalenceeffectamongnonexpertsdiagnosis |