Cargando…

Minimal representations of possibility at age 3

Young children do not always consider alternative possibilities when planning. Suppose a prize is hidden in a single occluded container and another prize is hidden in an occluded pair. If given a chance to choose one container and receive its contents, choosing the singleton maximizes expected rewar...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Leahy, Brian, Huemer, Michael, Steele, Matt, Alderete, Stephanie, Carey, Susan
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: National Academy of Sciences 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9907095/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36534794
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2207499119
_version_ 1784884102892093440
author Leahy, Brian
Huemer, Michael
Steele, Matt
Alderete, Stephanie
Carey, Susan
author_facet Leahy, Brian
Huemer, Michael
Steele, Matt
Alderete, Stephanie
Carey, Susan
author_sort Leahy, Brian
collection PubMed
description Young children do not always consider alternative possibilities when planning. Suppose a prize is hidden in a single occluded container and another prize is hidden in an occluded pair. If given a chance to choose one container and receive its contents, choosing the singleton maximizes expected reward because each member of the pair might be empty. Yet, 3-y-olds choose a member of the pair almost half the time. Why don’t they maximize expected reward? Three studies provide evidence that 3-y-olds do not deploy possibility concepts like MIGHT, which would let them represent that each container in the pair might and might not contain a prize. Rather, they build an overly specific model of the situation that correctly specifies that the singleton holds a prize while inappropriately specifying which member of the pair holds a prize and which is empty. So, when asked to choose a container, they see two equally good options. This predicts approximately 50% choice of the singleton, observed in studies 1 and 3. But when asked to throw away a container so that they can receive the remaining contents (study 2), they mostly throw away a member of the pair. The full pattern of data is expected if children construct overly specific models. We discuss whether 3-year-olds lack possibility concepts or whether performance demands prevent deployment of them in our tasks.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9907095
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher National Academy of Sciences
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-99070952023-02-08 Minimal representations of possibility at age 3 Leahy, Brian Huemer, Michael Steele, Matt Alderete, Stephanie Carey, Susan Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A Social Sciences Young children do not always consider alternative possibilities when planning. Suppose a prize is hidden in a single occluded container and another prize is hidden in an occluded pair. If given a chance to choose one container and receive its contents, choosing the singleton maximizes expected reward because each member of the pair might be empty. Yet, 3-y-olds choose a member of the pair almost half the time. Why don’t they maximize expected reward? Three studies provide evidence that 3-y-olds do not deploy possibility concepts like MIGHT, which would let them represent that each container in the pair might and might not contain a prize. Rather, they build an overly specific model of the situation that correctly specifies that the singleton holds a prize while inappropriately specifying which member of the pair holds a prize and which is empty. So, when asked to choose a container, they see two equally good options. This predicts approximately 50% choice of the singleton, observed in studies 1 and 3. But when asked to throw away a container so that they can receive the remaining contents (study 2), they mostly throw away a member of the pair. The full pattern of data is expected if children construct overly specific models. We discuss whether 3-year-olds lack possibility concepts or whether performance demands prevent deployment of them in our tasks. National Academy of Sciences 2022-12-19 2022-12-27 /pmc/articles/PMC9907095/ /pubmed/36534794 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2207499119 Text en Copyright © 2022 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY) (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) .
spellingShingle Social Sciences
Leahy, Brian
Huemer, Michael
Steele, Matt
Alderete, Stephanie
Carey, Susan
Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title_full Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title_fullStr Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title_full_unstemmed Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title_short Minimal representations of possibility at age 3
title_sort minimal representations of possibility at age 3
topic Social Sciences
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9907095/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36534794
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2207499119
work_keys_str_mv AT leahybrian minimalrepresentationsofpossibilityatage3
AT huemermichael minimalrepresentationsofpossibilityatage3
AT steelematt minimalrepresentationsofpossibilityatage3
AT alderetestephanie minimalrepresentationsofpossibilityatage3
AT careysusan minimalrepresentationsofpossibilityatage3