Cargando…
Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit
INTRODUCTION: Numerous previous studies have shown that eye movements induce errors in the localization of briefly flashed stimuli. Remarkably, the error pattern is indicative of the underlying eye movement and the exact experimental condition. For smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) and the slow ph...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2023
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9932511/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36816133 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1058340 |
_version_ | 1784889468719726592 |
---|---|
author | Dowiasch, Stefan Blanke, Marius Knöll, Jonas Bremmer, Frank |
author_facet | Dowiasch, Stefan Blanke, Marius Knöll, Jonas Bremmer, Frank |
author_sort | Dowiasch, Stefan |
collection | PubMed |
description | INTRODUCTION: Numerous previous studies have shown that eye movements induce errors in the localization of briefly flashed stimuli. Remarkably, the error pattern is indicative of the underlying eye movement and the exact experimental condition. For smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) and the slow phase of the optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), perceived stimulus locations are shifted in the direction of the ongoing eye movement, with a hemifield asymmetry observed only during SPEM. During the slow phases of the optokinetic afternystagmus (OKAN), however, the error pattern can be described as a perceptual expansion of space. Different from SPEM and OKN, the OKAN is an open-loop eye movement. METHODS: Visually guided smooth pursuit can be transformed into an open–loop eye movement by briefly blanking the pursuit target (gap). Here, we examined flash localization during open-loop pursuit and asked, whether localization is also prone to errors and whether these are similar to those found during SPEM or during OKAN. Human subjects tracked a pursuit target. In half of the trials, the target was extinguished for 300 ms (gap) during the steady–state, inducing open–loop pursuit. Flashes were presented during this gap or during steady–state (closed–loop) pursuit. RESULTS: In both conditions, perceived flash locations were shifted in the direction of the eye movement. The overall error pattern was very similar with error size being slightly smaller in the gap condition. The differences between errors in the open- and closed-loop conditions were largest in the central visual field and smallest in the periphery. DISCUSSION: We discuss the findings in light of the neural substrates driving the different forms of eye movements. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9932511 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2023 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-99325112023-02-17 Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit Dowiasch, Stefan Blanke, Marius Knöll, Jonas Bremmer, Frank Front Neurosci Neuroscience INTRODUCTION: Numerous previous studies have shown that eye movements induce errors in the localization of briefly flashed stimuli. Remarkably, the error pattern is indicative of the underlying eye movement and the exact experimental condition. For smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) and the slow phase of the optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), perceived stimulus locations are shifted in the direction of the ongoing eye movement, with a hemifield asymmetry observed only during SPEM. During the slow phases of the optokinetic afternystagmus (OKAN), however, the error pattern can be described as a perceptual expansion of space. Different from SPEM and OKN, the OKAN is an open-loop eye movement. METHODS: Visually guided smooth pursuit can be transformed into an open–loop eye movement by briefly blanking the pursuit target (gap). Here, we examined flash localization during open-loop pursuit and asked, whether localization is also prone to errors and whether these are similar to those found during SPEM or during OKAN. Human subjects tracked a pursuit target. In half of the trials, the target was extinguished for 300 ms (gap) during the steady–state, inducing open–loop pursuit. Flashes were presented during this gap or during steady–state (closed–loop) pursuit. RESULTS: In both conditions, perceived flash locations were shifted in the direction of the eye movement. The overall error pattern was very similar with error size being slightly smaller in the gap condition. The differences between errors in the open- and closed-loop conditions were largest in the central visual field and smallest in the periphery. DISCUSSION: We discuss the findings in light of the neural substrates driving the different forms of eye movements. Frontiers Media S.A. 2023-02-02 /pmc/articles/PMC9932511/ /pubmed/36816133 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1058340 Text en Copyright © 2023 Dowiasch, Blanke, Knöll and Bremmer. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
spellingShingle | Neuroscience Dowiasch, Stefan Blanke, Marius Knöll, Jonas Bremmer, Frank Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title | Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title_full | Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title_fullStr | Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title_full_unstemmed | Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title_short | Spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
title_sort | spatial localization during open-loop smooth pursuit |
topic | Neuroscience |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9932511/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36816133 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1058340 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT dowiaschstefan spatiallocalizationduringopenloopsmoothpursuit AT blankemarius spatiallocalizationduringopenloopsmoothpursuit AT knolljonas spatiallocalizationduringopenloopsmoothpursuit AT bremmerfrank spatiallocalizationduringopenloopsmoothpursuit |