Cargando…

People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood

Past work showed a tendency to associate verbal probabilities (e.g., possible, unlikely) with extreme quantitative outcomes, and to over-estimate the outcomes’ probability of occurrence. In the first four experiments (Experiment 1, Experiments 2a–c), we tested whether this “extremity effect” reflect...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Juanchich, Marie, Sirota, Miroslav, Halvor Teigen, Karl
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: SAGE Publications 2023
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10585946/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36645086
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17470218231153394
_version_ 1785123057027776512
author Juanchich, Marie
Sirota, Miroslav
Halvor Teigen, Karl
author_facet Juanchich, Marie
Sirota, Miroslav
Halvor Teigen, Karl
author_sort Juanchich, Marie
collection PubMed
description Past work showed a tendency to associate verbal probabilities (e.g., possible, unlikely) with extreme quantitative outcomes, and to over-estimate the outcomes’ probability of occurrence. In the first four experiments (Experiment 1, Experiments 2a–c), we tested whether this “extremity effect” reflects a general preference for extreme (vs central or less extreme) values of a distribution. Participants made predictions based on a frequency distribution in two scenarios. We did not find a preference for extreme outcomes. Instead, most of the participants made a prediction about the middle, most frequent outcome of the distribution (i.e., the modal outcome), but still over-estimated the outcomes’ probabilities. In Experiment 3, we tested whether the over-estimation could be better explained by an “at least”/“at most” reading of the predictions. We found that only a minority of participants interpreted predictions as the lower/upper bounds of an open interval and that these interpretations were not associated with heightened probability estimates. In the final three experiments (Experiments 4a–c), we tested whether participants perceived extreme outcome predictions as more correct, useful and interesting than modal outcome predictions. We found that extreme and modal predictions were considered equally correct, but modal predictions were judged most useful, whereas extreme predictions were judged to be more interesting. Overall, our results indicate that the preference for extreme outcomes is limited to specific verbal probability expressions, whereas the over-estimation of the probability of quantitative outcomes may be more general than anticipated and applies to non-extreme values as well.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-10585946
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2023
publisher SAGE Publications
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-105859462023-10-20 People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood Juanchich, Marie Sirota, Miroslav Halvor Teigen, Karl Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) Original Articles Past work showed a tendency to associate verbal probabilities (e.g., possible, unlikely) with extreme quantitative outcomes, and to over-estimate the outcomes’ probability of occurrence. In the first four experiments (Experiment 1, Experiments 2a–c), we tested whether this “extremity effect” reflects a general preference for extreme (vs central or less extreme) values of a distribution. Participants made predictions based on a frequency distribution in two scenarios. We did not find a preference for extreme outcomes. Instead, most of the participants made a prediction about the middle, most frequent outcome of the distribution (i.e., the modal outcome), but still over-estimated the outcomes’ probabilities. In Experiment 3, we tested whether the over-estimation could be better explained by an “at least”/“at most” reading of the predictions. We found that only a minority of participants interpreted predictions as the lower/upper bounds of an open interval and that these interpretations were not associated with heightened probability estimates. In the final three experiments (Experiments 4a–c), we tested whether participants perceived extreme outcome predictions as more correct, useful and interesting than modal outcome predictions. We found that extreme and modal predictions were considered equally correct, but modal predictions were judged most useful, whereas extreme predictions were judged to be more interesting. Overall, our results indicate that the preference for extreme outcomes is limited to specific verbal probability expressions, whereas the over-estimation of the probability of quantitative outcomes may be more general than anticipated and applies to non-extreme values as well. SAGE Publications 2023-02-18 2023-11 /pmc/articles/PMC10585946/ /pubmed/36645086 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17470218231153394 Text en © Experimental Psychology Society 2023 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
spellingShingle Original Articles
Juanchich, Marie
Sirota, Miroslav
Halvor Teigen, Karl
People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title_full People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title_fullStr People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title_full_unstemmed People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title_short People prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
title_sort people prefer to predict middle, most likely quantitative outcomes (not extreme ones), but they still over-estimate their likelihood
topic Original Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10585946/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36645086
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/17470218231153394
work_keys_str_mv AT juanchichmarie peopleprefertopredictmiddlemostlikelyquantitativeoutcomesnotextremeonesbuttheystilloverestimatetheirlikelihood
AT sirotamiroslav peopleprefertopredictmiddlemostlikelyquantitativeoutcomesnotextremeonesbuttheystilloverestimatetheirlikelihood
AT halvorteigenkarl peopleprefertopredictmiddlemostlikelyquantitativeoutcomesnotextremeonesbuttheystilloverestimatetheirlikelihood