Cargando…
How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study
A paradox at the heart of language acquisition research is that, to achieve adult-like competence, children must acquire the ability to generalize verbs into non-attested structures, while avoiding utterances that are deemed ungrammatical by native speakers. For example, children must learn that, to...
Autor principal: | |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3644877/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23252958 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12018 |
_version_ | 1782268497829560320 |
---|---|
author | Ambridge, Ben |
author_facet | Ambridge, Ben |
author_sort | Ambridge, Ben |
collection | PubMed |
description | A paradox at the heart of language acquisition research is that, to achieve adult-like competence, children must acquire the ability to generalize verbs into non-attested structures, while avoiding utterances that are deemed ungrammatical by native speakers. For example, children must learn that, to denote the reversal of an action, un- can be added to many verbs, but not all (e.g., roll/unroll; close/*unclose). This study compared theoretical accounts of how this is done. Children aged 5–6 (N = 18), 9–10 (N = 18), and adults (N = 18) rated the acceptability of un- prefixed forms of 48 verbs (and, as a control, bare forms). Across verbs, a negative correlation was observed between the acceptability of ungrammatical un- prefixed forms (e.g., *unclose) and the frequency of (a) the bare form and (b) alternative forms (e.g., open), supporting the entrenchment and pre-emption hypotheses, respectively. Independent ratings of the extent to which verbs instantiate the semantic properties characteristic of a hypothesized semantic cryptotype for un- prefixation were a significant positive predictor of acceptability, for all age groups. The relative importance of each factor differed for attested and unattested un- forms and also varied with age. The findings are interpreted in the context of a new hybrid account designed to incorporate the three factors of entrenchment, pre-emption, and verb semantics. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-3644877 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2013 |
publisher | Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-36448772013-05-06 How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study Ambridge, Ben Cogn Sci Regular Articles A paradox at the heart of language acquisition research is that, to achieve adult-like competence, children must acquire the ability to generalize verbs into non-attested structures, while avoiding utterances that are deemed ungrammatical by native speakers. For example, children must learn that, to denote the reversal of an action, un- can be added to many verbs, but not all (e.g., roll/unroll; close/*unclose). This study compared theoretical accounts of how this is done. Children aged 5–6 (N = 18), 9–10 (N = 18), and adults (N = 18) rated the acceptability of un- prefixed forms of 48 verbs (and, as a control, bare forms). Across verbs, a negative correlation was observed between the acceptability of ungrammatical un- prefixed forms (e.g., *unclose) and the frequency of (a) the bare form and (b) alternative forms (e.g., open), supporting the entrenchment and pre-emption hypotheses, respectively. Independent ratings of the extent to which verbs instantiate the semantic properties characteristic of a hypothesized semantic cryptotype for un- prefixation were a significant positive predictor of acceptability, for all age groups. The relative importance of each factor differed for attested and unattested un- forms and also varied with age. The findings are interpreted in the context of a new hybrid account designed to incorporate the three factors of entrenchment, pre-emption, and verb semantics. Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2013-04 2012-12-18 /pmc/articles/PMC3644877/ /pubmed/23252958 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12018 Text en Copyright © 2013 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/ Re-use of this article is permitted in accordance with the Creative Commons Deed, Attribution 2.5, which does not permit commercial exploitation. |
spellingShingle | Regular Articles Ambridge, Ben How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title | How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title_full | How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title_fullStr | How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title_full_unstemmed | How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title_short | How Do Children Restrict Their Linguistic Generalizations? An (Un-)Grammaticality Judgment Study |
title_sort | how do children restrict their linguistic generalizations? an (un-)grammaticality judgment study |
topic | Regular Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3644877/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23252958 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12018 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT ambridgeben howdochildrenrestricttheirlinguisticgeneralizationsanungrammaticalityjudgmentstudy |