Cargando…

Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?

In three studies, we explored the retention and transfer of tool-making knowledge, learnt from an adult demonstration, to other temporal and task contexts. All studies used a variation of a task in which children had to make a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tall transparent tube. Children who...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Beck, Sarah R., Cutting, Nicola, Apperly, Ian A., Demery, Zoe, Iliffe, Leila, Rishi, Sonia, Chappell, Jackie
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2014
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4255490/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25538650
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395
_version_ 1782347437714702336
author Beck, Sarah R.
Cutting, Nicola
Apperly, Ian A.
Demery, Zoe
Iliffe, Leila
Rishi, Sonia
Chappell, Jackie
author_facet Beck, Sarah R.
Cutting, Nicola
Apperly, Ian A.
Demery, Zoe
Iliffe, Leila
Rishi, Sonia
Chappell, Jackie
author_sort Beck, Sarah R.
collection PubMed
description In three studies, we explored the retention and transfer of tool-making knowledge, learnt from an adult demonstration, to other temporal and task contexts. All studies used a variation of a task in which children had to make a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tall transparent tube. Children who failed to innovate the hook tool independently saw a demonstration. In Study 1, we tested children aged 4–6 years (N = 53) who had seen the original demonstration 3 months earlier. Performance was excellent at the second time, indicating that children’s knowledge was retained over the 3 month period. In Studies 2 and 3 we explored transfer of the new knowledge to other tasks. In Study 2, children were given two variants of the apparatus that differed in surface characteristics (e.g., shape and color). Participants generalized their knowledge to these new apparatuses even though the new pipecleaner also differed in size and color. Five- to 6-year-olds (N = 22) almost always transferred their knowledge to problems where the same tool had to be made. Younger, 3- to 5-year-olds’ (N = 46), performance was more variable. In Study 3, 4- to 7-year-olds (N = 146) saw a demonstration of hook making with a pipecleaner, but then had to make a tool by combining pieces of wooden dowel (or vice versa: original training on dowel, transfer to pipecleaner). Children did not transfer their tool-making knowledge to the new material. Children retained tool-making knowledge over time and transferred their knowledge to new situations in which they needed to make a similar tool from similar materials, but not different materials. We concluded that children’s ability to use tool-making knowledge in novel situations is likely to depend on memory and analogical reasoning, with the latter continuing to develop during middle childhood.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-4255490
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2014
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-42554902014-12-23 Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems? Beck, Sarah R. Cutting, Nicola Apperly, Ian A. Demery, Zoe Iliffe, Leila Rishi, Sonia Chappell, Jackie Front Psychol Psychology In three studies, we explored the retention and transfer of tool-making knowledge, learnt from an adult demonstration, to other temporal and task contexts. All studies used a variation of a task in which children had to make a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tall transparent tube. Children who failed to innovate the hook tool independently saw a demonstration. In Study 1, we tested children aged 4–6 years (N = 53) who had seen the original demonstration 3 months earlier. Performance was excellent at the second time, indicating that children’s knowledge was retained over the 3 month period. In Studies 2 and 3 we explored transfer of the new knowledge to other tasks. In Study 2, children were given two variants of the apparatus that differed in surface characteristics (e.g., shape and color). Participants generalized their knowledge to these new apparatuses even though the new pipecleaner also differed in size and color. Five- to 6-year-olds (N = 22) almost always transferred their knowledge to problems where the same tool had to be made. Younger, 3- to 5-year-olds’ (N = 46), performance was more variable. In Study 3, 4- to 7-year-olds (N = 146) saw a demonstration of hook making with a pipecleaner, but then had to make a tool by combining pieces of wooden dowel (or vice versa: original training on dowel, transfer to pipecleaner). Children did not transfer their tool-making knowledge to the new material. Children retained tool-making knowledge over time and transferred their knowledge to new situations in which they needed to make a similar tool from similar materials, but not different materials. We concluded that children’s ability to use tool-making knowledge in novel situations is likely to depend on memory and analogical reasoning, with the latter continuing to develop during middle childhood. Frontiers Media S.A. 2014-12-04 /pmc/articles/PMC4255490/ /pubmed/25538650 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395 Text en Copyright © 2014 Beck, Cutting, Apperly, Demery, Iliffe, Rishi and Chappell. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Beck, Sarah R.
Cutting, Nicola
Apperly, Ian A.
Demery, Zoe
Iliffe, Leila
Rishi, Sonia
Chappell, Jackie
Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title_full Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title_fullStr Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title_full_unstemmed Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title_short Is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
title_sort is tool-making knowledge robust over time and across problems?
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4255490/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25538650
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01395
work_keys_str_mv AT becksarahr istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT cuttingnicola istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT apperlyiana istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT demeryzoe istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT iliffeleila istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT rishisonia istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems
AT chappelljackie istoolmakingknowledgerobustovertimeandacrossproblems