Cargando…

Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor

Accumulating evidence indicates high risk of bias in preclinical animal research, questioning the scientific validity and reproducibility of published research findings. Systematic reviews found low rates of reporting of measures against risks of bias in the published literature (e.g., randomization...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Vogt, Lucile, Reichlin, Thomas S., Nathues, Christina, Würbel, Hanno
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2016
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5135031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27911892
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000598
_version_ 1782471556912381952
author Vogt, Lucile
Reichlin, Thomas S.
Nathues, Christina
Würbel, Hanno
author_facet Vogt, Lucile
Reichlin, Thomas S.
Nathues, Christina
Würbel, Hanno
author_sort Vogt, Lucile
collection PubMed
description Accumulating evidence indicates high risk of bias in preclinical animal research, questioning the scientific validity and reproducibility of published research findings. Systematic reviews found low rates of reporting of measures against risks of bias in the published literature (e.g., randomization, blinding, sample size calculation) and a correlation between low reporting rates and inflated treatment effects. That most animal research undergoes peer review or ethical review would offer the possibility to detect risks of bias at an earlier stage, before the research has been conducted. For example, in Switzerland, animal experiments are licensed based on a detailed description of the study protocol and a harm–benefit analysis. We therefore screened applications for animal experiments submitted to Swiss authorities (n = 1,277) for the rates at which the use of seven basic measures against bias (allocation concealment, blinding, randomization, sample size calculation, inclusion/exclusion criteria, primary outcome variable, and statistical analysis plan) were described and compared them with the reporting rates of the same measures in a representative sub-sample of publications (n = 50) resulting from studies described in these applications. Measures against bias were described at very low rates, ranging on average from 2.4% for statistical analysis plan to 19% for primary outcome variable in applications for animal experiments, and from 0.0% for sample size calculation to 34% for statistical analysis plan in publications from these experiments. Calculating an internal validity score (IVS) based on the proportion of the seven measures against bias, we found a weak positive correlation between the IVS of applications and that of publications (Spearman’s rho = 0.34, p = 0.014), indicating that the rates of description of these measures in applications partly predict their rates of reporting in publications. These results indicate that the authorities licensing animal experiments are lacking important information about experimental conduct that determines the scientific validity of the findings, which may be critical for the weight attributed to the benefit of the research in the harm–benefit analysis. Similar to manuscripts getting accepted for publication despite poor reporting of measures against bias, applications for animal experiments may often be approved based on implicit confidence rather than explicit evidence of scientific rigor. Our findings shed serious doubt on the current authorization procedure for animal experiments, as well as the peer-review process for scientific publications, which in the long run may undermine the credibility of research. Developing existing authorization procedures that are already in place in many countries towards a preregistration system for animal research is one promising way to reform the system. This would not only benefit the scientific validity of findings from animal experiments but also help to avoid unnecessary harm to animals for inconclusive research.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5135031
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2016
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-51350312016-12-21 Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor Vogt, Lucile Reichlin, Thomas S. Nathues, Christina Würbel, Hanno PLoS Biol Meta-Research Article Accumulating evidence indicates high risk of bias in preclinical animal research, questioning the scientific validity and reproducibility of published research findings. Systematic reviews found low rates of reporting of measures against risks of bias in the published literature (e.g., randomization, blinding, sample size calculation) and a correlation between low reporting rates and inflated treatment effects. That most animal research undergoes peer review or ethical review would offer the possibility to detect risks of bias at an earlier stage, before the research has been conducted. For example, in Switzerland, animal experiments are licensed based on a detailed description of the study protocol and a harm–benefit analysis. We therefore screened applications for animal experiments submitted to Swiss authorities (n = 1,277) for the rates at which the use of seven basic measures against bias (allocation concealment, blinding, randomization, sample size calculation, inclusion/exclusion criteria, primary outcome variable, and statistical analysis plan) were described and compared them with the reporting rates of the same measures in a representative sub-sample of publications (n = 50) resulting from studies described in these applications. Measures against bias were described at very low rates, ranging on average from 2.4% for statistical analysis plan to 19% for primary outcome variable in applications for animal experiments, and from 0.0% for sample size calculation to 34% for statistical analysis plan in publications from these experiments. Calculating an internal validity score (IVS) based on the proportion of the seven measures against bias, we found a weak positive correlation between the IVS of applications and that of publications (Spearman’s rho = 0.34, p = 0.014), indicating that the rates of description of these measures in applications partly predict their rates of reporting in publications. These results indicate that the authorities licensing animal experiments are lacking important information about experimental conduct that determines the scientific validity of the findings, which may be critical for the weight attributed to the benefit of the research in the harm–benefit analysis. Similar to manuscripts getting accepted for publication despite poor reporting of measures against bias, applications for animal experiments may often be approved based on implicit confidence rather than explicit evidence of scientific rigor. Our findings shed serious doubt on the current authorization procedure for animal experiments, as well as the peer-review process for scientific publications, which in the long run may undermine the credibility of research. Developing existing authorization procedures that are already in place in many countries towards a preregistration system for animal research is one promising way to reform the system. This would not only benefit the scientific validity of findings from animal experiments but also help to avoid unnecessary harm to animals for inconclusive research. Public Library of Science 2016-12-02 /pmc/articles/PMC5135031/ /pubmed/27911892 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000598 Text en © 2016 Vogt et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Meta-Research Article
Vogt, Lucile
Reichlin, Thomas S.
Nathues, Christina
Würbel, Hanno
Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title_full Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title_fullStr Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title_full_unstemmed Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title_short Authorization of Animal Experiments Is Based on Confidence Rather than Evidence of Scientific Rigor
title_sort authorization of animal experiments is based on confidence rather than evidence of scientific rigor
topic Meta-Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5135031/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27911892
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.2000598
work_keys_str_mv AT vogtlucile authorizationofanimalexperimentsisbasedonconfidenceratherthanevidenceofscientificrigor
AT reichlinthomass authorizationofanimalexperimentsisbasedonconfidenceratherthanevidenceofscientificrigor
AT nathueschristina authorizationofanimalexperimentsisbasedonconfidenceratherthanevidenceofscientificrigor
AT wurbelhanno authorizationofanimalexperimentsisbasedonconfidenceratherthanevidenceofscientificrigor