Cargando…
The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (H...
Autores principales: | , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do
Comportamento
2009
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5618977/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29213632 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008 |
_version_ | 1783267304457371648 |
---|---|
author | Nunes, Maria Vania Silva Castro-Caldas, Alexandre Rio, Dolores Del Maestú, Fernado Ortiz, Tomás |
author_facet | Nunes, Maria Vania Silva Castro-Caldas, Alexandre Rio, Dolores Del Maestú, Fernado Ortiz, Tomás |
author_sort | Nunes, Maria Vania Silva |
collection | PubMed |
description | The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. METHODS: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. RESULTS: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. CONCLUSIONS: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5618977 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2009 |
publisher | Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do
Comportamento |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-56189772017-12-06 The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model Nunes, Maria Vania Silva Castro-Caldas, Alexandre Rio, Dolores Del Maestú, Fernado Ortiz, Tomás Dement Neuropsychol Original Articles The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. METHODS: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. RESULTS: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. CONCLUSIONS: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task. Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento 2009 /pmc/articles/PMC5618977/ /pubmed/29213632 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008 Text en http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
spellingShingle | Original Articles Nunes, Maria Vania Silva Castro-Caldas, Alexandre Rio, Dolores Del Maestú, Fernado Ortiz, Tomás The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model |
title | The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and
HAROLD model |
title_full | The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and
HAROLD model |
title_fullStr | The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and
HAROLD model |
title_full_unstemmed | The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and
HAROLD model |
title_short | The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and
HAROLD model |
title_sort | ex-illiterate brain: the critical period, cognitive reserve and
harold model |
topic | Original Articles |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5618977/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29213632 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT nunesmariavaniasilva theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT castrocaldasalexandre theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT riodoloresdel theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT maestufernado theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT ortiztomas theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT nunesmariavaniasilva exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT castrocaldasalexandre exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT riodoloresdel exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT maestufernado exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel AT ortiztomas exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel |