Cargando…

The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model

The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (H...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Nunes, Maria Vania Silva, Castro-Caldas, Alexandre, Rio, Dolores Del, Maestú, Fernado, Ortiz, Tomás
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento 2009
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5618977/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29213632
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008
_version_ 1783267304457371648
author Nunes, Maria Vania Silva
Castro-Caldas, Alexandre
Rio, Dolores Del
Maestú, Fernado
Ortiz, Tomás
author_facet Nunes, Maria Vania Silva
Castro-Caldas, Alexandre
Rio, Dolores Del
Maestú, Fernado
Ortiz, Tomás
author_sort Nunes, Maria Vania Silva
collection PubMed
description The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. METHODS: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. RESULTS: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. CONCLUSIONS: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5618977
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2009
publisher Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-56189772017-12-06 The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model Nunes, Maria Vania Silva Castro-Caldas, Alexandre Rio, Dolores Del Maestú, Fernado Ortiz, Tomás Dement Neuropsychol Original Articles The lifelong acquisition of cognitive skills shapes the biology of the brain. However, there are critical periods for the best use of the brain to process the acquired information. OBJECTIVES: To discuss the critical period of cognitive acquisition, the concept of cognitive reserve and the HAROLD (Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older adults) model. METHODS: Seven women who learned how to read and to write after the age of 50 (ex-illiterates) and five women with 10 years of regular schooling (controls) were submitted to a language recognition test while brain activity was being recorded using magnetoencephalography. Spoken words were delivered binaurally via two plastic tubs terminating in ear inserts, and recordings were made with a whole head magnetometer consisting of 148 magnetometer coils. RESULTS: Both groups performed similarly on the task of identifying target words. Analysis of the number of sources of activity in the left and right hemispheres revealed significant differences between the two groups, showing that ex-illiterate subjects exhibited less brain functional asymmetry during the language task. CONCLUSIONS: These results should be interpreted with caution because the groups were small. However, these findings reinforce the concept that poorly educated subjects tend to use the brain for information processing in a different way to subjects with a high educational level or who were schooled at the regular time. Finally, the recruiting of both hemispheres to tackle the language recognition test occurred to a greater degree in the ex-illiterate group where this can be interpreted as a sign of difficulty performing the task. Associação de Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento 2009 /pmc/articles/PMC5618977/ /pubmed/29213632 http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008 Text en http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
spellingShingle Original Articles
Nunes, Maria Vania Silva
Castro-Caldas, Alexandre
Rio, Dolores Del
Maestú, Fernado
Ortiz, Tomás
The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_full The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_fullStr The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_full_unstemmed The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_short The ex-illiterate brain: The critical period, cognitive reserve and HAROLD model
title_sort ex-illiterate brain: the critical period, cognitive reserve and harold model
topic Original Articles
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5618977/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29213632
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30300008
work_keys_str_mv AT nunesmariavaniasilva theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT castrocaldasalexandre theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT riodoloresdel theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT maestufernado theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT ortiztomas theexilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT nunesmariavaniasilva exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT castrocaldasalexandre exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT riodoloresdel exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT maestufernado exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel
AT ortiztomas exilliteratebrainthecriticalperiodcognitivereserveandharoldmodel