Cargando…
Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning
These studies explore elementary-school-aged children’s ability to evaluate circular explanations and whether they respond to receiving weak explanations by expressing interest in additional learning. In the first study, 6-, 8-, and 10-year-olds (n = 53) heard why questions about unfamiliar animals....
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
Springer US
2017
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5643350/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28176292 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1195-2 |
_version_ | 1783271512477794304 |
---|---|
author | Mills, Candice M. Danovitch, Judith H. Rowles, Sydney P. Campbell, Ian L. |
author_facet | Mills, Candice M. Danovitch, Judith H. Rowles, Sydney P. Campbell, Ian L. |
author_sort | Mills, Candice M. |
collection | PubMed |
description | These studies explore elementary-school-aged children’s ability to evaluate circular explanations and whether they respond to receiving weak explanations by expressing interest in additional learning. In the first study, 6-, 8-, and 10-year-olds (n = 53) heard why questions about unfamiliar animals. For each question, they rated the quality of single explanations and later selected the best explanation between pairs of circular and noncircular explanations. When judging single explanations, 8- and 10-year-olds, and to some extent 6-year-olds, provided higher ratings for noncircular explanations compared to circular ones. When selecting between pairs of explanations, all age groups preferred noncircular explanations to circular ones, but older children did so more consistently than 6-year-olds. Children who recognized the weakness of the single circular explanations were more interested in receiving additional information about the question topics. In Study 2, all three age groups (n = 87) provided higher ratings for noncircular explanations compared to circular ones when listening to responses to how questions, but older children showed a greater distinction in their ratings than 6-year-olds. Moreover, the link between recognizing circular explanations as weak and interest in future learning could not be accounted for solely by individual differences in verbal intelligence. These findings illustrate the developmental trajectory of explanation evaluation and support that recognition of weak explanations is linked to interest in future learning across the elementary years. Implications for education are discussed. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-5643350 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | Springer US |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-56433502017-10-27 Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning Mills, Candice M. Danovitch, Judith H. Rowles, Sydney P. Campbell, Ian L. Psychon Bull Rev Brief Report These studies explore elementary-school-aged children’s ability to evaluate circular explanations and whether they respond to receiving weak explanations by expressing interest in additional learning. In the first study, 6-, 8-, and 10-year-olds (n = 53) heard why questions about unfamiliar animals. For each question, they rated the quality of single explanations and later selected the best explanation between pairs of circular and noncircular explanations. When judging single explanations, 8- and 10-year-olds, and to some extent 6-year-olds, provided higher ratings for noncircular explanations compared to circular ones. When selecting between pairs of explanations, all age groups preferred noncircular explanations to circular ones, but older children did so more consistently than 6-year-olds. Children who recognized the weakness of the single circular explanations were more interested in receiving additional information about the question topics. In Study 2, all three age groups (n = 87) provided higher ratings for noncircular explanations compared to circular ones when listening to responses to how questions, but older children showed a greater distinction in their ratings than 6-year-olds. Moreover, the link between recognizing circular explanations as weak and interest in future learning could not be accounted for solely by individual differences in verbal intelligence. These findings illustrate the developmental trajectory of explanation evaluation and support that recognition of weak explanations is linked to interest in future learning across the elementary years. Implications for education are discussed. Springer US 2017-02-07 2017 /pmc/articles/PMC5643350/ /pubmed/28176292 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1195-2 Text en © The Author(s) 2016 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. |
spellingShingle | Brief Report Mills, Candice M. Danovitch, Judith H. Rowles, Sydney P. Campbell, Ian L. Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title | Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title_full | Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title_fullStr | Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title_full_unstemmed | Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title_short | Children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
title_sort | children’s success at detecting circular explanations and their interest in future learning |
topic | Brief Report |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5643350/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28176292 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1195-2 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT millscandicem childrenssuccessatdetectingcircularexplanationsandtheirinterestinfuturelearning AT danovitchjudithh childrenssuccessatdetectingcircularexplanationsandtheirinterestinfuturelearning AT rowlessydneyp childrenssuccessatdetectingcircularexplanationsandtheirinterestinfuturelearning AT campbellianl childrenssuccessatdetectingcircularexplanationsandtheirinterestinfuturelearning |