Cargando…

Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty

An eye-tracking methodology was used to explore adults’ and children’s use of two utterance-based cues to overcome referential uncertainty in real time. Participants were first introduced to two characters with distinct color preferences. These characters then produced fluent (“Look! Look at the bli...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Thacker, Justine M., Chambers, Craig G., Graham, Susan A.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5816787/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29487559
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00143
_version_ 1783300746910892032
author Thacker, Justine M.
Chambers, Craig G.
Graham, Susan A.
author_facet Thacker, Justine M.
Chambers, Craig G.
Graham, Susan A.
author_sort Thacker, Justine M.
collection PubMed
description An eye-tracking methodology was used to explore adults’ and children’s use of two utterance-based cues to overcome referential uncertainty in real time. Participants were first introduced to two characters with distinct color preferences. These characters then produced fluent (“Look! Look at the blicket.”) or disfluent (“Look! Look at thee, uh, blicket.”) instructions referring to novel objects in a display containing both talker-preferred and talker-dispreferred colored items. Adults (Expt 1, n = 24) directed a greater proportion of looks to talker-preferred objects during the initial portion of the utterance (“Look! Look at…”), reflecting the use of indexical cues for talker identity. However, they immediately reduced consideration of an object bearing the talker’s preferred color when the talker was disfluent, suggesting they infer disfluency would be more likely as a talker describes dispreferred objects. Like adults, 5-year-olds (Expt 2, n = 27) directed more attention to talker-preferred objects during the initial portion of the utterance. Children’s initial predictions, however, were not modulated when disfluency was encountered. Together, these results demonstrate that adults, but not 5-year-olds, can act on information from two talker-produced cues within an utterance, talker preference, and speech disfluencies, to establish reference.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-5816787
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-58167872018-02-27 Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty Thacker, Justine M. Chambers, Craig G. Graham, Susan A. Front Psychol Psychology An eye-tracking methodology was used to explore adults’ and children’s use of two utterance-based cues to overcome referential uncertainty in real time. Participants were first introduced to two characters with distinct color preferences. These characters then produced fluent (“Look! Look at the blicket.”) or disfluent (“Look! Look at thee, uh, blicket.”) instructions referring to novel objects in a display containing both talker-preferred and talker-dispreferred colored items. Adults (Expt 1, n = 24) directed a greater proportion of looks to talker-preferred objects during the initial portion of the utterance (“Look! Look at…”), reflecting the use of indexical cues for talker identity. However, they immediately reduced consideration of an object bearing the talker’s preferred color when the talker was disfluent, suggesting they infer disfluency would be more likely as a talker describes dispreferred objects. Like adults, 5-year-olds (Expt 2, n = 27) directed more attention to talker-preferred objects during the initial portion of the utterance. Children’s initial predictions, however, were not modulated when disfluency was encountered. Together, these results demonstrate that adults, but not 5-year-olds, can act on information from two talker-produced cues within an utterance, talker preference, and speech disfluencies, to establish reference. Frontiers Media S.A. 2018-02-13 /pmc/articles/PMC5816787/ /pubmed/29487559 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00143 Text en Copyright © 2018 Thacker, Chambers and Graham. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Thacker, Justine M.
Chambers, Craig G.
Graham, Susan A.
Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title_full Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title_fullStr Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title_full_unstemmed Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title_short Five-Year-Olds’ and Adults’ Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty
title_sort five-year-olds’ and adults’ use of paralinguistic cues to overcome referential uncertainty
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5816787/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29487559
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00143
work_keys_str_mv AT thackerjustinem fiveyearoldsandadultsuseofparalinguisticcuestoovercomereferentialuncertainty
AT chamberscraigg fiveyearoldsandadultsuseofparalinguisticcuestoovercomereferentialuncertainty
AT grahamsusana fiveyearoldsandadultsuseofparalinguisticcuestoovercomereferentialuncertainty