Cargando…

A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom

Children from low SES backgrounds hear, on average, fewer words at home than those from high SES backgrounds. This word gap is associated with widening achievement differences in children’s language abilities and school readiness. However relatively little is known about adult and child speech in ch...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Perry, Lynn K., Prince, Emily B., Valtierra, Adriana M., Rivero-Fernandez, Camila, Ullery, Mary Anne, Katz, Lynne F., Laursen, Brett, Messinger, Daniel S.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Public Library of Science 2018
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6034821/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29979740
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199893
_version_ 1783337941708308480
author Perry, Lynn K.
Prince, Emily B.
Valtierra, Adriana M.
Rivero-Fernandez, Camila
Ullery, Mary Anne
Katz, Lynne F.
Laursen, Brett
Messinger, Daniel S.
author_facet Perry, Lynn K.
Prince, Emily B.
Valtierra, Adriana M.
Rivero-Fernandez, Camila
Ullery, Mary Anne
Katz, Lynne F.
Laursen, Brett
Messinger, Daniel S.
author_sort Perry, Lynn K.
collection PubMed
description Children from low SES backgrounds hear, on average, fewer words at home than those from high SES backgrounds. This word gap is associated with widening achievement differences in children’s language abilities and school readiness. However relatively little is known about adult and child speech in childcare settings, in which approximately 30% of American children are enrolled. We examined the influence of teacher and peer language input on children’s in-class language use and language development in an intervention classroom for low-SES, high-risk 2- to 3-year-olds. Over the course of a year, day-long recordings of the classroom were collected weekly with LENA recorders. Using LENA software algorithms, we found that language input from peers was positively related to children’s in-class language use, both in-the-moment and over the course of each day, as were the number of conversational turns in which children and teachers engaged Both peer input and conversational turns with teachers were also positively related to children’s language development rates, as indexed by increases in vocabulary size. Together these results indicate the importance of child-specific rates of classroom language input in the language development of high-risk, preschoolers.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6034821
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2018
publisher Public Library of Science
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-60348212018-07-19 A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom Perry, Lynn K. Prince, Emily B. Valtierra, Adriana M. Rivero-Fernandez, Camila Ullery, Mary Anne Katz, Lynne F. Laursen, Brett Messinger, Daniel S. PLoS One Research Article Children from low SES backgrounds hear, on average, fewer words at home than those from high SES backgrounds. This word gap is associated with widening achievement differences in children’s language abilities and school readiness. However relatively little is known about adult and child speech in childcare settings, in which approximately 30% of American children are enrolled. We examined the influence of teacher and peer language input on children’s in-class language use and language development in an intervention classroom for low-SES, high-risk 2- to 3-year-olds. Over the course of a year, day-long recordings of the classroom were collected weekly with LENA recorders. Using LENA software algorithms, we found that language input from peers was positively related to children’s in-class language use, both in-the-moment and over the course of each day, as were the number of conversational turns in which children and teachers engaged Both peer input and conversational turns with teachers were also positively related to children’s language development rates, as indexed by increases in vocabulary size. Together these results indicate the importance of child-specific rates of classroom language input in the language development of high-risk, preschoolers. Public Library of Science 2018-07-06 /pmc/articles/PMC6034821/ /pubmed/29979740 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199893 Text en © 2018 Perry et al http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
spellingShingle Research Article
Perry, Lynn K.
Prince, Emily B.
Valtierra, Adriana M.
Rivero-Fernandez, Camila
Ullery, Mary Anne
Katz, Lynne F.
Laursen, Brett
Messinger, Daniel S.
A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title_full A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title_fullStr A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title_full_unstemmed A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title_short A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
title_sort year in words: the dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom
topic Research Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6034821/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29979740
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199893
work_keys_str_mv AT perrylynnk ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT princeemilyb ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT valtierraadrianam ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT riverofernandezcamila ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT ullerymaryanne ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT katzlynnef ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT laursenbrett ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT messingerdaniels ayearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT perrylynnk yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT princeemilyb yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT valtierraadrianam yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT riverofernandezcamila yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT ullerymaryanne yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT katzlynnef yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT laursenbrett yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom
AT messingerdaniels yearinwordsthedynamicsandconsequencesoflanguageexperiencesinaninterventionclassroom