Cargando…

Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?

The purpose of the study was to investigate cross-language effects in verbal fluency tasks where participants name in English as many exemplars of a target as they can in one minute. A series of multiple regression models were used that employed predictors such as self-rated proficiency in English,...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Paap, Kenneth R., Mason, Lauren A., Zimiga, Brandon M., Ayala-Silva, Yocelyne, Frost, Matthew M., Gonzalez, Melissa, Primero, Lesley
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: MDPI 2019
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6721414/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31344826
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9080175
_version_ 1783448337855283200
author Paap, Kenneth R.
Mason, Lauren A.
Zimiga, Brandon M.
Ayala-Silva, Yocelyne
Frost, Matthew M.
Gonzalez, Melissa
Primero, Lesley
author_facet Paap, Kenneth R.
Mason, Lauren A.
Zimiga, Brandon M.
Ayala-Silva, Yocelyne
Frost, Matthew M.
Gonzalez, Melissa
Primero, Lesley
author_sort Paap, Kenneth R.
collection PubMed
description The purpose of the study was to investigate cross-language effects in verbal fluency tasks where participants name in English as many exemplars of a target as they can in one minute. A series of multiple regression models were used that employed predictors such as self-rated proficiency in English, self-rated proficiency in a language other than English, a picture naming task used to measure productive vocabulary, the percentage of English use, and the frequency of language switching. The main findings showed that self-rated proficiency in the non-English language accounted for unique variance in verbal fluency that was not accounted for directly by self-rated proficiency in English. This outcome is consistent with cross-language interference, but is also consistent with an account that assumes bilingual disadvantages in verbal fluency and picture naming are due to bilinguals having weaker links between semantic concepts and their phonological form. The present study is also discussed in terms of a broader framework that questions whether domain-general inhibition exists and also whether it plays an important role in bilingual language control.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-6721414
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2019
publisher MDPI
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-67214142019-09-10 Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference? Paap, Kenneth R. Mason, Lauren A. Zimiga, Brandon M. Ayala-Silva, Yocelyne Frost, Matthew M. Gonzalez, Melissa Primero, Lesley Brain Sci Article The purpose of the study was to investigate cross-language effects in verbal fluency tasks where participants name in English as many exemplars of a target as they can in one minute. A series of multiple regression models were used that employed predictors such as self-rated proficiency in English, self-rated proficiency in a language other than English, a picture naming task used to measure productive vocabulary, the percentage of English use, and the frequency of language switching. The main findings showed that self-rated proficiency in the non-English language accounted for unique variance in verbal fluency that was not accounted for directly by self-rated proficiency in English. This outcome is consistent with cross-language interference, but is also consistent with an account that assumes bilingual disadvantages in verbal fluency and picture naming are due to bilinguals having weaker links between semantic concepts and their phonological form. The present study is also discussed in terms of a broader framework that questions whether domain-general inhibition exists and also whether it plays an important role in bilingual language control. MDPI 2019-07-24 /pmc/articles/PMC6721414/ /pubmed/31344826 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9080175 Text en © 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
Paap, Kenneth R.
Mason, Lauren A.
Zimiga, Brandon M.
Ayala-Silva, Yocelyne
Frost, Matthew M.
Gonzalez, Melissa
Primero, Lesley
Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title_full Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title_fullStr Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title_full_unstemmed Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title_short Other Language Proficiency Predicts Unique Variance in Verbal Fluency Not Accounted for Directly by Target Language Proficiency: Cross-Language Interference?
title_sort other language proficiency predicts unique variance in verbal fluency not accounted for directly by target language proficiency: cross-language interference?
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6721414/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31344826
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9080175
work_keys_str_mv AT paapkennethr otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT masonlaurena otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT zimigabrandonm otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT ayalasilvayocelyne otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT frostmatthewm otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT gonzalezmelissa otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference
AT primerolesley otherlanguageproficiencypredictsuniquevarianceinverbalfluencynotaccountedfordirectlybytargetlanguageproficiencycrosslanguageinterference