Cargando…

Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices

Food-specific inhibition training (FSIT) is a computerised task requiring response inhibition to energy-dense foods within a reaction-time game. Previous work indicates that FSIT can increase the number of healthy foods (relative to energy-dense foods) children choose, and decrease calories consumed...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Porter, Lucy, Gillison, Fiona B., Wright, Kim A., Verbruggen, Frederick, Lawrence, Natalia S.
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: Frontiers Media S.A. 2021
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8161504/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34054657
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653610
_version_ 1783700525958561792
author Porter, Lucy
Gillison, Fiona B.
Wright, Kim A.
Verbruggen, Frederick
Lawrence, Natalia S.
author_facet Porter, Lucy
Gillison, Fiona B.
Wright, Kim A.
Verbruggen, Frederick
Lawrence, Natalia S.
author_sort Porter, Lucy
collection PubMed
description Food-specific inhibition training (FSIT) is a computerised task requiring response inhibition to energy-dense foods within a reaction-time game. Previous work indicates that FSIT can increase the number of healthy foods (relative to energy-dense foods) children choose, and decrease calories consumed from sweets and chocolate. Across two studies, we explored the impact of FSIT variations (e.g., different response signals, different delivery modes) on children’s food choices within a time-limited hypothetical food-choice task. In Study 1, we varied the FSIT Go/No-Go signals to be emotive (happy vs. sad faces) or neutral (green vs. red signs). One-hundred-and-fifty-seven children were randomly allocated to emotive-FSIT, neutral-FSIT, or a non-food control task. Children participated in groups of 4–15. No significant FSIT effects were observed on food choices (all values of p > 0.160). Healthy-food choices decreased over time regardless of condition (p < 0.050). The non-significant effects could be explained by lower accuracy on energy-dense No-Go trials than in previous studies, possibly due to distraction in the group-testing environment. In Study 2, we compared computer-based FSIT (using emotive signals) and app-based FSIT (using neutral signals) against a non-food control with a different sample of 206 children, but this time children worked one-on-one with the experimenter. Children’s accuracy on energy-dense No-Go trials was higher in this study. Children in the FSIT-computer group chose significantly more healthy foods at post-training (M = 2.78, SE = 0.16) compared to the control group (M = 2.02, SE = 0.16, p = 0.001). The FSIT-app group did not differ from either of the other two groups (M = 2.42, SE = 0.16, both comparisons p > 0.050). Healthy choices decreased over time in the control group (p = 0.001) but did not change in the two FSIT groups (both p > 0.300) supporting previous evidence that FSIT may have a beneficial effect on children’s food choices. Ensuring that children perform FSIT with high accuracy (e.g., by using FSIT in quiet environments and avoiding group-testing) may be important for impacts on food choices though. Future research should continue to explore methods of optimising FSIT as a healthy-eating intervention for children.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-8161504
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2021
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-81615042021-05-29 Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices Porter, Lucy Gillison, Fiona B. Wright, Kim A. Verbruggen, Frederick Lawrence, Natalia S. Front Psychol Psychology Food-specific inhibition training (FSIT) is a computerised task requiring response inhibition to energy-dense foods within a reaction-time game. Previous work indicates that FSIT can increase the number of healthy foods (relative to energy-dense foods) children choose, and decrease calories consumed from sweets and chocolate. Across two studies, we explored the impact of FSIT variations (e.g., different response signals, different delivery modes) on children’s food choices within a time-limited hypothetical food-choice task. In Study 1, we varied the FSIT Go/No-Go signals to be emotive (happy vs. sad faces) or neutral (green vs. red signs). One-hundred-and-fifty-seven children were randomly allocated to emotive-FSIT, neutral-FSIT, or a non-food control task. Children participated in groups of 4–15. No significant FSIT effects were observed on food choices (all values of p > 0.160). Healthy-food choices decreased over time regardless of condition (p < 0.050). The non-significant effects could be explained by lower accuracy on energy-dense No-Go trials than in previous studies, possibly due to distraction in the group-testing environment. In Study 2, we compared computer-based FSIT (using emotive signals) and app-based FSIT (using neutral signals) against a non-food control with a different sample of 206 children, but this time children worked one-on-one with the experimenter. Children’s accuracy on energy-dense No-Go trials was higher in this study. Children in the FSIT-computer group chose significantly more healthy foods at post-training (M = 2.78, SE = 0.16) compared to the control group (M = 2.02, SE = 0.16, p = 0.001). The FSIT-app group did not differ from either of the other two groups (M = 2.42, SE = 0.16, both comparisons p > 0.050). Healthy choices decreased over time in the control group (p = 0.001) but did not change in the two FSIT groups (both p > 0.300) supporting previous evidence that FSIT may have a beneficial effect on children’s food choices. Ensuring that children perform FSIT with high accuracy (e.g., by using FSIT in quiet environments and avoiding group-testing) may be important for impacts on food choices though. Future research should continue to explore methods of optimising FSIT as a healthy-eating intervention for children. Frontiers Media S.A. 2021-05-13 /pmc/articles/PMC8161504/ /pubmed/34054657 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653610 Text en Copyright © 2021 Porter, Gillison, Wright, Verbruggen and Lawrence. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
spellingShingle Psychology
Porter, Lucy
Gillison, Fiona B.
Wright, Kim A.
Verbruggen, Frederick
Lawrence, Natalia S.
Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title_full Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title_fullStr Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title_full_unstemmed Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title_short Exploring Strategies to Optimise the Impact of Food-Specific Inhibition Training on Children’s Food Choices
title_sort exploring strategies to optimise the impact of food-specific inhibition training on children’s food choices
topic Psychology
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8161504/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34054657
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653610
work_keys_str_mv AT porterlucy exploringstrategiestooptimisetheimpactoffoodspecificinhibitiontrainingonchildrensfoodchoices
AT gillisonfionab exploringstrategiestooptimisetheimpactoffoodspecificinhibitiontrainingonchildrensfoodchoices
AT wrightkima exploringstrategiestooptimisetheimpactoffoodspecificinhibitiontrainingonchildrensfoodchoices
AT verbruggenfrederick exploringstrategiestooptimisetheimpactoffoodspecificinhibitiontrainingonchildrensfoodchoices
AT lawrencenatalias exploringstrategiestooptimisetheimpactoffoodspecificinhibitiontrainingonchildrensfoodchoices