Cargando…

Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type

The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both...

Descripción completa

Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: van Hoogmoed, Anne H., Wegman, Joost, van den Brink, Danielle, Janzen, Gabriele
Formato: Online Artículo Texto
Lenguaje:English
Publicado: MDPI 2022
Materias:
Acceso en línea:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9221540/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741661
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776
_version_ 1784732648592113664
author van Hoogmoed, Anne H.
Wegman, Joost
van den Brink, Danielle
Janzen, Gabriele
author_facet van Hoogmoed, Anne H.
Wegman, Joost
van den Brink, Danielle
Janzen, Gabriele
author_sort van Hoogmoed, Anne H.
collection PubMed
description The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both categorical performance, indicating the notion of location changes based on the landmarks, as well as metrical performance relating to the precision of the representation of the environment. We investigated whether age, sex, spatial working memory, verbal working memory, and verbal production of left and right contributed to the development of navigation skills. In egocentric navigation, Categorical performance was already above chance at 5 years of age and was positively related to visuo-spatial working memory and the production of left/right, whereas metrical performance was only related to age. Allocentric navigation started to develop between 5 and 8 years of age and was related to sex, with boys outperforming girls. Both boys and girls seemed to rely more on directional landmark information as compared to positional landmark information. To our knowledge, this study is the first to give insight into the relative contribution of different cognitive abilities to navigation skills in school-aged children.
format Online
Article
Text
id pubmed-9221540
institution National Center for Biotechnology Information
language English
publishDate 2022
publisher MDPI
record_format MEDLINE/PubMed
spelling pubmed-92215402022-06-24 Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type van Hoogmoed, Anne H. Wegman, Joost van den Brink, Danielle Janzen, Gabriele Brain Sci Article The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both categorical performance, indicating the notion of location changes based on the landmarks, as well as metrical performance relating to the precision of the representation of the environment. We investigated whether age, sex, spatial working memory, verbal working memory, and verbal production of left and right contributed to the development of navigation skills. In egocentric navigation, Categorical performance was already above chance at 5 years of age and was positively related to visuo-spatial working memory and the production of left/right, whereas metrical performance was only related to age. Allocentric navigation started to develop between 5 and 8 years of age and was related to sex, with boys outperforming girls. Both boys and girls seemed to rely more on directional landmark information as compared to positional landmark information. To our knowledge, this study is the first to give insight into the relative contribution of different cognitive abilities to navigation skills in school-aged children. MDPI 2022-06-13 /pmc/articles/PMC9221540/ /pubmed/35741661 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776 Text en © 2022 by the authors. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
spellingShingle Article
van Hoogmoed, Anne H.
Wegman, Joost
van den Brink, Danielle
Janzen, Gabriele
Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title_full Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title_fullStr Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title_full_unstemmed Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title_short Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
title_sort development of landmark use for navigation in children: effects of age, sex, working memory and landmark type
topic Article
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9221540/
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741661
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776
work_keys_str_mv AT vanhoogmoedanneh developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype
AT wegmanjoost developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype
AT vandenbrinkdanielle developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype
AT janzengabriele developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype