Cargando…
Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both...
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | Online Artículo Texto |
Lenguaje: | English |
Publicado: |
MDPI
2022
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9221540/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741661 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776 |
_version_ | 1784732648592113664 |
---|---|
author | van Hoogmoed, Anne H. Wegman, Joost van den Brink, Danielle Janzen, Gabriele |
author_facet | van Hoogmoed, Anne H. Wegman, Joost van den Brink, Danielle Janzen, Gabriele |
author_sort | van Hoogmoed, Anne H. |
collection | PubMed |
description | The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both categorical performance, indicating the notion of location changes based on the landmarks, as well as metrical performance relating to the precision of the representation of the environment. We investigated whether age, sex, spatial working memory, verbal working memory, and verbal production of left and right contributed to the development of navigation skills. In egocentric navigation, Categorical performance was already above chance at 5 years of age and was positively related to visuo-spatial working memory and the production of left/right, whereas metrical performance was only related to age. Allocentric navigation started to develop between 5 and 8 years of age and was related to sex, with boys outperforming girls. Both boys and girls seemed to rely more on directional landmark information as compared to positional landmark information. To our knowledge, this study is the first to give insight into the relative contribution of different cognitive abilities to navigation skills in school-aged children. |
format | Online Article Text |
id | pubmed-9221540 |
institution | National Center for Biotechnology Information |
language | English |
publishDate | 2022 |
publisher | MDPI |
record_format | MEDLINE/PubMed |
spelling | pubmed-92215402022-06-24 Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type van Hoogmoed, Anne H. Wegman, Joost van den Brink, Danielle Janzen, Gabriele Brain Sci Article The use of landmarks for navigation develops throughout childhood. Here, we examined the developmental trajectory of egocentric and allocentric navigation based on landmark information in an on-screen virtual environment in 39 5–6-year-olds, 43 7–8-year-olds, and 41 9–10-year-olds. We assessed both categorical performance, indicating the notion of location changes based on the landmarks, as well as metrical performance relating to the precision of the representation of the environment. We investigated whether age, sex, spatial working memory, verbal working memory, and verbal production of left and right contributed to the development of navigation skills. In egocentric navigation, Categorical performance was already above chance at 5 years of age and was positively related to visuo-spatial working memory and the production of left/right, whereas metrical performance was only related to age. Allocentric navigation started to develop between 5 and 8 years of age and was related to sex, with boys outperforming girls. Both boys and girls seemed to rely more on directional landmark information as compared to positional landmark information. To our knowledge, this study is the first to give insight into the relative contribution of different cognitive abilities to navigation skills in school-aged children. MDPI 2022-06-13 /pmc/articles/PMC9221540/ /pubmed/35741661 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776 Text en © 2022 by the authors. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). |
spellingShingle | Article van Hoogmoed, Anne H. Wegman, Joost van den Brink, Danielle Janzen, Gabriele Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title | Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title_full | Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title_fullStr | Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title_full_unstemmed | Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title_short | Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type |
title_sort | development of landmark use for navigation in children: effects of age, sex, working memory and landmark type |
topic | Article |
url | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9221540/ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35741661 http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060776 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT vanhoogmoedanneh developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype AT wegmanjoost developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype AT vandenbrinkdanielle developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype AT janzengabriele developmentoflandmarkusefornavigationinchildreneffectsofagesexworkingmemoryandlandmarktype |